rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 1:199 (September 1993)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<1:199>From HOLSINGE@UCONNVM.BITNET Fri Sep 24 07:44:59 1993 Date: Fri, 24 Sep 1993 08:25:42 -0500 (EST) From: "Kent E. Holsinger" <HOLSINGE%UCONNVM.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: Re: Heritability and cultural evolution To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Gerson makes a reasonable point in his reply to Burian. Using the term "heritability" to refer to both biological transmission and cultural transmission runs a risk. By failing to distinguish between them, on the basis of the _very_ different mechanisms underlying them, we may unwittingly make use of evolutionary principles that depend on biological heredity when trying to understand cultural evolution. It is important, however, to realize that there will be _some_ commonality between the processes, no matter how different the mechanisms underlying the transmission. Darwin's theory of natural selection, for example, requires only that offspring resemble their parents, i.e., that there is a _correlation_ between parental and offspring phenotypes. It does _not_ require any that any particular mode of transmission underly that correlation. In fact, Darwin got the mode of inheritance completely wrong. To the extent that we can make inferences about the characteristics of an evolutionary process from the fact of transmission alone, there are bound to be similarities between biological and cultural evolution. There are two aspects of cultural transmission that seem to have no counterpart in biological transmission (that I have been able to think of, at least). First, transmission isn't strictly unidirectional, from parent to offspring, in cultural transmission. Second, there is considerable horizontal transmission among individuals. Take attitudes towards homosexuality as an example. (I should note before proceeding that this entire discussion is based only on my _perception_ of attitudes, not on any actual data about them. Still, it serves to illustrate the point.) Attitudes among college-age students towards homosexuality seem clearly influenced by the environment in which they were raised, i.e., by their parents (at least in part). That's classical vertical transmission. However, students' attitudes are also influenced by the attitudes and behavior of their peers. I'm sure we all know of cases where a student who was adamantly anti-homosexual discovers that a friend is gay and, as a result, changes his attitude about homsexual behavior. (Of course, many times attitudes don't change.) That's an example of horizontal transmission. Similarly, parents sometimes change their attitudes about homosexual behavior as a result of learning that their son or daughter is gay/lesbian/bisexual. That's reverse vertical transmission. One of the significant questions in my mind is whether the extent of these alternative modes of transmission is so great that cultural and biological evolution share few interesting properties or if their extent is limited enough that there are significant similarities. Another is whether there are certain classes of cultural evolution, e.g., linguistic evolution, that are more similar to biological evolution than others, e.g., changes in sexual mores. -- Kent +--------------------------------------------------------------------+ | Kent E. Holsinger Internet: Holsinge@UConnVM.UConn.edu | | Dept. of Ecology & BITNET: Holsinge@UConnVM | | Evolutionary Biology, U-43 | | University of Connecticut | | Storrs, CT 06269-3043 | +--------------------------------------------------------------------+
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!