rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 1:276 (September 1993)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<1:276>From sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu Thu Sep 30 17:56:03 1993 To: Gregory Mayer <mayerg@cs.uwp.edu> Subject: Re: Heritability and cultural evolution Date: Thu, 30 Sep 93 18:58:27 -0400 From: Sally Thomason <sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu> What historical linguists mean by "structural imbalances" is parts of the system that are irregular and therefore harder to learn than regular things. The rubric can (with some stretching) be extended to cover *all* things that are relatively hard to learn, but the real meaning has to do with irregularities -- patterns with gaps, patterns with exceptions, that sort of thing. I was using it in a pretty casual way, to avoid giving lists of the kinds of things that are hard to learn; sorry to be obscure. Maybe an example will help clarify the notion. For instance: In Serbo-Croatian (a Slavic language, the major language of ex-Yugoslavia), a few hundred years ago, two vowels merged into one, pronounced [i] (= the vowel in English _beet_). But this merger caused problems in the system of noun declension, because a previously regular rule that turned (for instance) [k] into a [ts] sound originally applied before only one of the vowels that merged; after the merger, [k]'s appearing before the [i]'s that used to trigger the [k] --> [ts] rule still turned to [ts], but [k]'s appearing before the vowel that originally did NOT trigger the rule remained [k]. If a noun stem ended in [k], speakers had to remember which particular suffixes beginning in [i] triggered the rule and which ones didn't. That's an imbalance in the system. So what happened? Different dialects of the language did different things: (a) Some dialects just lost the rule, so that all [k]'s remained [k] before all [i]'s; (b) some dialects kept the old rule, so speakers had to memorize the particular suffixes beginning in [i] that triggered the rule; (c) some dialects extended the rule to apply before *all* [i]'s, both the [i]'s that had originally triggered the process and the [i]'s from an original vowel that did not trigger the process. These constituted other imbalances (well, the same imbalance, in the case of (b)). For instance, the (c) case fixed the noun declensional system, but introduced a discrepancy between noun declension and verb declension, where the [k] --> [ts] rule still occurred before certain suffixes. Language is, according to a popular truism, a system of systems, and all these systems interact in complex ways. So (another truism!) a change that regularizes one (part of one) subsystem is all too likely to complicate some part of the same or another subsystem. What you find, therefore, is a never-ending process of change, fixing up a glitch here only to introduce a glitch there. Historical linguists aren't likely to find a punctuated equilibrium approach useful for the study of language change, in other words: no equilibrium. (And that in itself, come to think of it, makes our discipline unhospitable to the notion of a just-right adaptation to a just-right environment, even if we could decide what we mean by "environment".) Sally Thomason sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!