rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 4:7 (December 1993)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<4:7>From sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu Fri Dec 3 17:23:13 1993 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: linguistic drifts or "imbalances" Date: Fri, 03 Dec 93 18:26:41 -0500 From: Sally Thomason <sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu> The term "drift" is so vague that it isn't surprising if nonlinguists find it unclear; it isn't what Gerard Donnelly Smith has in mind, though. I haven't been keeping up with postings lately, so maybe other linguists have discussed drift already. I'm sorry if I'm repeating things. Imbalances (a loose term that is mainly an abbreviation for a lot of different structural conditions) in a linguistic system are, ultimately, things that are hard to learn because they're irregular, or hard to hear, or whatever. A hole in a pattern can also be an imbalance: an example that has , I think, been mentioned here before is the new second person plural pronoun that appears in various dialects -- not always the same form, but always the same function, to disambiguate 2nd person reference: yunz (here in Pittsburgh), y'all (for some Southern speakers), etc. But many structural imbalances are not gaps in patterns. Irregular forms, especially those that aren't very commonly used, are often vulnerable to regularization. So, for instance, most young Americans have only _dreamed_ as the past tense of _dream_; the new past is still competing with the old irregular past tense _dreamt_ for many English speakers. Another recent example of regularization is the plural of _cow_: now it's _cows_, but it used to be something like "ki" (I forget just what the vowel would have been in Modern English) -- or, rather, it would have been "ki" (or so) if _kine_ hadn't been formed instead, with the same suffix as in _oxen_. Still another type of "imbalance" is often considered a trigger for sound change -- things that are hard to hear, such as final syllables in long words which are stressed on the initial syllable. (Well, maybe I'm using the term "imbalance" more broadly than some other linguists might. My point is that the notion covers a heterogeneous bunch of phenomena. There are others besides the ones I've mentioned.) The idea behind drift, in historical linguistics, is this: if a language A is in the process of splitting, or has recently split, into two daughter languages B and C, B and C have of course inherited all the structures of A, including the hard-to-learn things -- patterns with gaps, hard-to-hear sounds, irregular forms, etc. Because they have inherited the same structures, and because changes that are motivated in part by difficulty of learning are often quite similar, we are likely to find a sizable number of identical, or very similar, changes in B and C. This fact has methodological implications. Our Comparative Method, by means of which we can reconstruct (parts of) an unattested, i.e. literally prehistoric, parent language of a family of related languages, assumes -- among other things -- that features shared by all the daughter languages can safely be reconstructed for the parent language. Obviously, the results of changes due to drift, that is, changes that occur AFTER B and C (the only daughter languages of A) have diverged independently from A, are not features that were present in A; so such features have the potential of misleading us into reconstructing things for A that A didn't have. Sometimes we can find evidence that will keep us from making this mistake: for instance, the so-called "yers" of late Proto-Slavic were extra-short vowels that underwent very similar changes in the various Slavic languages. But the results of those changes, while similar, differ in detail from language to language; so we know that they post-dated the splitting of Proto-Slavic into the various Slavic languages. Sometimes, though, we don't have such evidence from differences in the details of changes, and so we are bound to reconstruct some things for a proto-langauge (parent language of a family) that it didn't have. No way to check. And, of course, we are also unable to reconstruct some things for a proto-language that it DID have -- things that have been lost in all the daughter languages, maybe due to drift. Our methods have limitations. But isn't that true in all the historical sciences? Sally Thomason sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!