rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 4:9 (December 1993)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<4:9>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sat Dec 4 23:35:49 1993 Date: Sun, 05 Dec 1993 00:42:16 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Ancestral and derived states: interdisciplinary historical notes To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro Here are some notes on the history of the distinction between ancestral and derived character states in systematics and philology. I had intended to organize this material a bit better, but haven't had the time yet. This topic might in fact be a very nice one for a short historical paper. With that possibility in mind I'd be grateful for any additional references anyone may know that discuss the history of these ideas, particularly in philology. Systematics In contemporary systematics, the distinction between ancestral and derived character states is often traced to Hennig (1966), and while it is very true that this distinction didn't really catch fire until the 1960s, there are several few earlier examples of its use. Robin Craw has written a very nice paper (1992) that traces some of these earlier uses, as well as Hennig's modern influence. (It was Hennig who coined the terms "apomorphy" and "plesiomorphy".) The most thorough exposition of the ancestral/derived distinction I have seen in the early systematic literature comes from Peter Chalmers Mitchell; Craw discusses him, and I have also commented on his work (O'Hara, 1988). Here's a sample from one of Mitchell's later works: "Characters have to be judged as well as counted, if it be intended to use them for estimating the relative degree of affinity between animal types. No anatomist doubts but that Man retains many primitive characters; Anthropoid Apes, Old-World Monkeys, American Monkeys, Tarsius, and Lemurs also retain many primitive | characters. It is reasonable to assume that the common ancestors of all these animals possessed all the primitive characters retained by any of them. And so it is not surprising to find any primitive character in any descendant of a common stock, but there is no reason to suppose that, because any two have retained the same primitive character, they should for that reason be judged more nearly related than either may be with some other descendant of the common stock. Primitive characters may be useful for the description or definition of a group -- they have no value for assigning degrees of affinity. These considerations ought to be commonplaces in zoological argument, but they are often forgotten, and I think they have been entirely forgotten by Professor Wood-Jones in the imposing list of common characters that he has drawn up for Man and Tarsius. Fortunately they have been remembered by Mr. Pocock, and Professors Hill and Elliot Smith, and the considerations they adduce have disposed of Professor Wood-Jones's argument that Tarsius has special relation to the ancestry of Man. It may not be a Lemur, but it is no nearer to Man than to other Primates." (Mitchell, 1919:496-497) Philology In stemmatics (the reconstruction of manuscript genealogy) a derived character state is simply an "error", or more precisely an "indicative error", and the ancestral state is the sought-for "original reading". In historical linguistics a derived state is often called an "innovation", and an ancestral state a "retention". Here are a couple of extracts that comment on the ancestral/derived distinction in philology: "Some of these interdisciplinary influences [among the historical sciences], which are part of general intellectual history, were acknowledged explicitly, as when August Schleicher proclaimed himself a Darwinist of sorts (Hoenigswald 1963; Koerner 1978). Other influences -- probably far more genuine -- existed within the personalities of the practicioners in the form of well-assimilated modes of thinking, too deeply ingrained even to be specifically discussed, as when the same Schleicher transferred the principle of the exclusively shared copying error from manuscript work to linguistics, where it surfaced as the principle of shared innovation." (Hoenigswald, 1990:442) "In 1884 came K. Brugmann's work in which we have the most influential (cf. Dyen 1953, 1978), though not the first, statement of the requirement that account be taken not of shared properties (which could, after all, be retained properties) but of shared innovations -- a requirement, as we have seen...with its own historical interest, doctrinal as well as practical. It has sometimes been held (Watkins 1966; Markey 1976) that signigicant retentions should be accorded the same standing. We must, however, remember that retentions and innovations are not independent phenomena but converses. An innovation is a non-retention, and while shared retentions are compatible with a subgrouping, innovations are indicative of one." (Hoenigswald, 1990:443) References Cited Craw, Robin. 1992. Margins of cladistics: identity, difference and place in the emergence of phylogenetic systemaitcs, 1864-1975. Pp. 65-107 in: Trees of Life: Essays in Philosophy of Biology (Paul Griffiths, ed.). Australasian Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 11. Dyen, Isidore. 1953. Review of Malgache et Manjaan by Otto Ch. Dahl. Language, 29:577-590. Dyen, Isidore. 1978. Subgrouping and reconstruction. Pp. 33-52 in: Linguistic and Literary Studies in Honor of Archibald A. Hill (M. A. Jazayeri, E. Polome, & W. Winter, eds.). Vol. II. The Hague: Mouton. Hennig, Willi. 1966. Phylogenetic Systematics. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. (Original edition in German, 1950.) Hoenigswald, Henry M. 1963. On the history of the comparative method. Anthropological Linguistics, 5:1-11. Hoenigswald, Henry M. 1990. Language families and subgroupings, tree model and wave theory, and reconstruction of protolanguages. Pp. 441-454 in: Research Guide on Language Change (Edgar C. Polome, ed.). Trends in Linguistics, Studies and Monographs, 48. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Koerner, E. F. Konrad. 1978. Toward a historiography of linguistics: 19th and 20th century paradigms. In: toward a Historiography of Linguistics: Selected Essays. Amsterdam Studies in the theory and History of Linguistic Science, III. Studies in the History of Linguistics, vol. 19. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Markey, Thomas L. Germanic Dialect Grouping and the Position of Ingvaeonic. Innsbrucker Beitrage zur Sprachwissenschaft 15. Innsbruck: Institut fur Sprachwissenschaft der Universitat Innsbruck. Mitchell, Peter Chalmers. 1919. [Discussion on the zoological position and affinities of Tarsius.] Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 1919:496-497. O'Hara, Robert J. 1988. Diagrammatic classifications of birds, 1819-1901: views of the natural system in 19th-century British ornithology. Pp. 2746- 2759 in: Acta XIX Congressus Internationalis Ornithologici (Henri Ouellet, ed.). Ottawa: National Museum of Natural Sciences. Watkins, Calvert. 1966. Italo-Celtic revisited. Pp. 29-50 in: Ancient Indo-European Dialects (Henrik Brinbaum & Jaan Puhvel, eds.). Berkeley: University of California Press. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A.
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!