rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 5:88 (January 1994)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<5:88>From delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Mon Jan 17 11:46:36 1994 Date: Mon, 17 Jan 1994 09:28:15 -0800 (PST) From: Scott C DeLancey <delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu> Subject: Re: Systematics and linguistics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Kent E. Holsinger pursues the question of parallels and non-parallels between biological systematics and genetic linguistics, describing a situation in which two subgroups of bats share "details of skeletal anatomy (and more recently from molecular sequences)", but one of them shows resemblances to primates in details of neural anatomy, creating a problem in classification. This is indeed reminiscent of the problem of a language like Japanese, with apparent affinities to both Altaic and Austronesian (which are themselves not related at any detectable level). I think there are differences, though, which are worth exploring (if only to see whether I misunderstand the biological case). Kent says: > I'm not sure what processes would produce convergence in vocabularies, but it > appears that the problem Scott DeLancey is describing is similar in many ways > to the one I've just described, except perhaps that linguists do not yet have > a comprehensive theory that they could use to explain the convergences. There is no imaginable process that would produce convergence in vocabularies. Similarities in vocabulary beyond what can be expected by chance can only reflect common inheritance (i.e. genetic relationship) or borrowing. Vocabulary and details of morphological structure (critically including not only patterns of structure but actual forms) play a role analagous to molecular sequences more than to morphological patterns, the difference being that vocabulary, at least, is very easily borrowed, even between unrelated languages. I see the linguistic analogue to the kinds of morphological similarities which can in principle be considered to reflect convergence in biology as being typological similarities--type (rather than details) of morphological structure, parallel syntactic constructions, case marking patterns, etc. Because they can so easily arise by "convergence" (though that term is not much used), these sorts of similarities carry very little weight in determining linguistic relationships. (I think historical linguists sometimes find biological arguments about cladistic vs. phenetic taxonomy confusing, since only cladistic classification has ever been recognized as a worthwhile or interesting pursuit in historical linguistics). Scott DeLancey delancey@darwking.uoregon.edu Department of Linguistics University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!