rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 5:90 (January 1994)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<5:90>From sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu Mon Jan 17 21:26:54 1994 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: Systematics and linguistics Date: Mon, 17 Jan 94 22:29:59 -0500 From: Sally Thomason <sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu> John Limber raises some interesting points wrt Scott DeLancey's claim that "There is no imaginable process that would produce convergence in vocabularies", but these points don't affect the validity of Scott's claim. Since a language is likely to have 20 or 30 phonemes (or more), favoring simple forms over complex ones or having shorter forms for more frequent concepts won't produce enough chance similarities to make any difference: the combinatory possibilities for 20 or 30 phonemes will be too great, even after you build in sequencing constraints (on consonant clusters and vowel sequences, for instance), in words of typical lengths -- at least 3 phonemes long, say. (Note too that acquisition processes can't favor simple forms over complex ones all the time, or words would simplify into minimality, which they clearly don't, in ANY language; only grammatical items like prepositions and other such particles are very likely to get reduced. Lexical items, such as nouns and verbs, are more likely to be stressed, and stress tends to protect a word from drastic phonetic reduction.) Phonetic symbolism is another matter: most linguists would agree that you might find occasional convergences due to sound symbolism. But the overall effect on the vocabulary from such a process will be slight: examples aren't all that easy to find, and the prospect of significant overhaul of any language's vocabulary through such a process is, well, unimaginable (in the real world of human language). Limber is right, however, to point to the slipperiness of the notion of "chance". Non-historically-connected similarities in parts of words, at least, do occur in quantities that make randomness unlikely. For instance, many languages have no syllables (and therefore no words) ending in consonants; every syllable ends in a vowel. Tongue-tip sounds like t d n r are very frequent at the ends of words, in a wide variety of unrelated languages, and they turn up in grammatical suffixes (e.g. case endings on nouns, person/number endings on verbs) in so many languages that one might suspect them of being historically stable, relatively speaking, in that position. Many languages lack consonant clusters, and those that have them are most likely to have only clusters like pr-, gl-, ty-, and the like, rather than "heavy" clusters like st-, tk-, etc. Tendencies like these cause linguists to think in terms like ease of pronunciation, and ease of learning more generally, in the search for explanations. But again, they don't justify any prediction about general vocabulary convergence, because there is no evidence at all that such a thing occurs, except -- in a sense -- through borrowing, and that isn't what evolutionary biologists mean by "convergence". Sally Thomason sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!