rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 5:108 (January 1994)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<5:108>From delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Wed Jan 19 13:05:15 1994 Date: Wed, 19 Jan 1994 11:01:44 -0800 (PST) From: Scott C DeLancey <delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu> Subject: Re: Systematics and linguistics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Kent Holsinger's summary this morning of the parallels we've been working on between biological and linguistic evolution pretty well captures what Sally and I have been saying. There are two points that haven't been foregrounded in the discussion, that could still use clarification: > 2) Convergence is more frequent (or at least more frequently invoked) in > biological evolution than in language evolution. I would make a gross analogical equation of corresponding morphemes (and hence words) in language to molecular sequences, and of typological similarities among languages to morphological similarities among species. The first criteria are the most compelling in both fields, because they cannot be the result of convergence. But while biologists have only (relatively) recently had molecular sequencing data of various kinds available, they have had to deal all along with morphological similarities and the related problem of convergence. Linguists, of course, have had word and morphological* comparisons as their primary data from the beginning of historical linguistics, and have never paid much attention to typological similarities--thus the problem of convergence doesn't come up much. > 3) In both biological systematics and historical linguistics resemblances > decay enough over time that it may become difficult (perhaps in the case > of languages, impossible) to identify historical relationships, even > though in both cases we are (reasonably) certain that our objects of study > all share a single common ancestor some time in the distant past. To use > cladistic terminology, both life and human languages are monophyletic. Actually this is a precarious assumption in linguistics. (I think this is where this discussion got started, but I'm not sure we ever came to grips with this issue). There are both biological and memetic aspects to the origin of language, and these need to be sorted out before we can get very far with the question of monogenesis vs. polygenesis. It's clear that there are some biological adaptations to linguistic behavior in humans, though there's bitter controversy about what kind and how extensive and specific they are. (Bitter in part because no one has anything really substantive to contribute to the issue). So presumably the initial development of linguistic behavior took place in a single population ancestral to all modern humans. But we have no clear idea how far that development went, and it remains conceivable (though IMO certainly the less likely hypothesis) that the development of what we would recognize as full-fledged language was a cultural rather than biological development which could have occurred more than once. Scott DeLancey delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Department of Linguistics University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403 *I hope everybody's easy with the fact that "morphological" means quite different things in linguistics and biology.
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!