rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 8:8 (April 1994)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<8:8>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Tue Apr 5 13:53:26 1994 Date: Tue, 05 Apr 1994 15:50:18 -0500 (EST) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: "Cladistics" and "typology" To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro This comes from Sally Thomason, who was having temporary mail problems. Bob O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) --------------------------------------- Bob O'Hara suspects that the eyes of us non-systematists will be glazing over at the discussion of what is & isn't cladistics, and why: not so, at least for this non-systematist -- I'm finding this discussion really educational, and if I keep reading long enough I might actually understand what's going on in that area. Understanding the biological systematists is crucial for figuring out the relationships between the tree-building enterprises in biology and linguistics (and elsewhere), after all. Bob's revised definition of cladistic analysis -- the method based on the idea that only innovations count in identifying clades -- is THE obvious link between systematics and historical linguistics: shared innovations has always ("always" = since the late decades of the 19th century, when the comparative method got into high gear) been the primary criterion for subgrouping languages in a family tree. (And since the comparative method, which most of us believe is the only tried & true method for establishing language families, hasn't been shown to be valid for time depths anywhere near the presumed origin of human language, our numerous language families are each, for all practical purposes, the equivalent of the systematists' single tree.) A brief comment on Bob's mention of the different connotations of typology in linguistics and systematics (and apologies if this repeats comments made in earlier discussions of typology): typology is absolutely respectable in linguistics, but NOT as a historical methodology. It is in a synchronic field of study that seeks to identify common, not so common, and nonexistent structural patterns in languages of the world, with a view toward understanding the functionally and/or genetically determined (or at least influenced) properties of human language. It's true that typological arguments have been brought to bear, by a number of scholars, on the question of what to reconstruct, for a parent language, from the patterns found in a bunch of daughter languages. One line of argumentation (maybe the main line) goes like this: (almost) no language in the world has pattern X; therefore, we cannot reconstruct pattern X for the ancestor of these languages. Arguments of this strong type are highly controversial and have not been generally accepted. The best- known instance, probably, is the Glottalic Theory, a typological proposal for the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European stops. But in a weaker form, typology IS used by all of us in reconstructing bits of proto- languages: when, for instance, you have two sounds that correspond regularly in the only two daughter languages of a family -- say, h and p -- and you have to make a guess about which (if either) of these two sounds represents the parent language's sound...or, more precisely, phoneme...you guess the p, essentially on typological grounds: we all know of quite a few sound changes of the type *p > h, but we know of few or no changes of the type *h > p; since, therefore, *p > h seems a more likely change, we reconstruct *p for the proto-language's phoneme. [Some historical linguist who is more knowledgeable than I am will promptly post fifteen examples of sound changes from *h to p! So I'll respond to that posting now: the methodological point is valid, even if the particular example is no good. Pretty weak answer, eh? But I think most historical linguists would agree with it.] Notice, though, that using typology in this way in reconstructing proto-languages has nothing to do with establishing language families. Probably no current historical linguist would argue that shared structural features in themselves provide evidence for language-family grouping. Sally Thomason sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!