rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 8:14 (April 1994)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<8:14>From sally@isp.pitt.edu Wed Apr 6 18:40:40 1994 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: "Cladistics" and "typology" Date: Wed, 06 Apr 94 20:40:37 -0400 From: Sally Thomason <sally@isp.pitt.edu> Peter Cannell asks what a language family is -- good question! I've been avoiding using our standard term, which is "genetic relationship", for fear of confusing and/or annoying people who deal with literal rather than metaphorical genes. Anyway, the concept of a language family is pretty clear, although -- not surprisingly -- it's fuzzy around the edges: if one language diverges over time into two or more daughter languages, that's a language family. The daughter languages are changed later forms of their single parent language. There are some methodological assumptions which, though clearly invalid most of the time in real life, are useful nevertheless; the major one is that, when a lg. splits into two or more daughters, the split is CLEAN: that is, no more contact between the daughters after the split. You don't get a split at all unless there's some breakdown/reduction in communication between speakers of two or more dialects; but cases like Romani, whose speakers had no more contact at all with other Indic languages after they left India in ?1500 A.D.?, are pretty rare, as far as we know. At shallow enough time depths, when there's still a lot of evidence left (not too many changes in the daughter languages to obscure the regularities of descendent lexical and grammatical features), it's fairly easy to separate inherited material from borrowed material (i.e. convergences), IFF the borrowing isn't occurring between very closely related languages. But that's at shallow enough time depths. Once you're several thousand years away from the source, things get more difficult; that's why historical linguists make an informal guess of ca. 10,000 years as the upper limit for establishing genetic relationships among languages. (Most historical linguists, anyway.) After that too many changes have left so few systematic traces that it can be impossible to distinguish borrowed from inherited features, even when you're pretty sure that there is SOME historical connection between two or more languages. So historical linguists don't expect ever to construct a family tree for ALL human languages, assuming that there was once one single original language; we have some hundreds of language families currently, and though that number will undoubtedly shrink as the laborious historical study of different groups progresses, it's unlikely to shrink to just a few huge language families -- not at the time depths that, to judge by the level of diversity in the world's several thousand languages, must be reckoned with. Language is significantly different from other cultural or behavioral studies, though: language change is largely, though not entirely, independent of speakers' or societies' desires and intentions; language changes willy-nilly, which is why (for instance) pundits who rail against the sloppiness of, say, modern English speakers have been thriving for hundreds of years. There are fads in language, to be sure (like teen-age slang), but they are quite superficial: the structure of the language tends not to be affected by fads. Other cultural and behavioral studies have nothing like the regularity hypothesis of sound change, which is our main means of tracing language history back through time. Sally Thomason sally@isp.pitt.edu
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!