rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 2: 90–122 — October 1993
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
Darwin-L was an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences, active from 1993–1997. Darwin-L was established to promote the reintegration of a range of fields all of which are concerned with reconstructing the past from evidence in the present, and to encourage communication among scholars, scientists, and researchers in these fields. The group had more than 600 members from 35 countries, and produced a consistently high level of discussion over its several years of operation. Darwin-L was not restricted to evolutionary biology nor to the work of Charles Darwin, but instead addressed the entire range of historical sciences from an explicitly comparative perspective, including evolutionary biology, historical linguistics, textual transmission and stemmatics, historical geology, systematics and phylogeny, archeology, paleontology, cosmology, historical geography, historical anthropology, and related “palaetiological” fields.
This log contains public messages posted to the Darwin-L discussion group during October 1993. It has been lightly edited for format: message numbers have been added for ease of reference, message headers have been trimmed, some irregular lines have been reformatted, and error messages and personal messages accidentally posted to the group as a whole have been deleted. No genuine editorial changes have been made to the content of any of the posts. This log is provided for personal reference and research purposes only, and none of the material contained herein should be published or quoted without the permission of the original poster.
The master copy of this log is maintained in the Darwin-L Archives (rjohara.net/darwin) by Dr. Robert J. O’Hara. The Darwin-L Archives also contain additional information about the Darwin-L discussion group, the complete Today in the Historical Sciences calendar for every month of the year, a collection of recommended readings on the historical sciences, and an account of William Whewell’s concept of “palaetiology.”
---------------------------------------------- DARWIN-L MESSAGE LOG 2: 90-122 -- OCTOBER 1993 ---------------------------------------------- DARWIN-L A Network Discussion Group on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu is an international network discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. Darwin-L was established in September 1993 to promote the reintegration of a range of fields all of which are concerned with reconstructing the past from evidence in the present, and to encourage communication among academic professionals in these fields. Darwin-L is not restricted to evolutionary biology nor to the work of Charles Darwin but instead addresses the entire range of historical sciences from an interdisciplinary perspective, including evolutionary biology, historical linguistics, textual transmission and stemmatics, historical geology, systematics and phylogeny, archeology, paleontology, cosmology, historical anthropology, historical geography, and related "palaetiological" fields. This log contains public messages posted to Darwin-L during October 1993. It has been lightly edited for format: message numbers have been added for ease of reference, message headers have been trimmed, some irregular lines have been reformatted, and some administrative messages and personal messages posted to the group as a whole have been deleted. No genuine editorial changes have been made to the content of any of the posts. This log is provided for personal reference and research purposes only, and none of the material contained herein should be published or quoted without the permission of the original poster. The master copy of this log is maintained in the archives of Darwin-L by listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. For instructions on how to retrieve copies of this and other log files, and for additional information about Darwin-L, send the e-mail message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. Darwin-L is administered by Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu), Center for Critical Inquiry in the Liberal Arts and Department of Biology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A., and it is supported by the Center for Critical Inquiry, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, and the Department of History and the Academic Computing Center, University of Kansas. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:90>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sat Oct 16 15:21:37 1993 Date: Sat, 16 Oct 1993 16:28:35 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: October 16 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro OCTOBER 16 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1714: GIOVANNI ARDUINO (or ARDUINI) is born at Caprino, Italy. As a young man he will work in the mines of the Adige valley, and will soon became one of the leading mining specialists in Italy. From his study of the mountains and plains of northern Italy Arduino will describe four general geological periods which he will name Primary, Secondary, Tertiary, and Quaternary. He will go on to lay the intellectual foundations for the principle of actualism which will become one of the central concepts of historical geology: "With the sole guidance of our practical knowledge of those physical agents which we see actually used in the continuous workings of nature, and of our knowledge of the respective effects induced by the same workings, we can with reasonable basis surmise what the forces were which acted even in the remotest times." Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc. ukans.edu, a network discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. E-mail darwin@iris.uncg.edu for more information. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:91>From ANWOLFE@ECUVM.CIS.ECU.EDU Mon Oct 18 10:36:03 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 93 11:37:56 EDT From: ANWOLFE@ECUVM.CIS.ECU.EDU Subject: Re: waterbabes To: Multiple recipients of list <darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu> For those interested in the the aquatic ape hypothesis should take a look at "The Aquatic Ape: Fact or Fiction?" 1991, Souvenir Press ed. by M Roede, J. Wind, J. Patrick, and V. Reynolds. Linda Wolfe _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:92>From LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU Mon Oct 18 11:04:37 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 1993 11:04:37 -0500 From: "JOHN LANGDON" <LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: waterbabes In message <9310160106.AA11055@ucmp1.Berkeley.EDU> writes: > I have been following (with about one eye) the aquatic ape thread on > sci.bio on Usenet. Most of the posts are devoted to yarn-spinning and > just-so stories pro and con. I do wonder how proponents of the aquatic > ape scenario would respond to the following: > > According to the DNA clock (and the figure on about p. 20 of _The Third > Chimpanzee_ by Jared Diamond), the split between the gorilla clade and > the clade (Homo,(chimpanzee,bonobo)) is set at about 1.7-2.0 Ma. So how > could climatic events in the late Miocene (ca. 7-5 Ma) have anything to > do with autapomorphies of Homo? > > Unless, of course, we are willing to admit massive and homoplastic > reversals in both the gorilla and (chimp,bonobo) clades ... I am not a defender of the aquatic hypothesis. However, the only sense I can make out of this question is that you misread the graph. Diamond shows the human/Pan split at about 7 Myr. (The 1.7-2.0 figure is the % DNA difference.) There is still enough uncertainty in this date that the strict chronology does not rule out an extremely (phenomonally) rapid aquatic adaptation, but one that effectively discards Australopithecus. However, the aquatic ape model (proposed 1960, developed 1972) makes the most sense under the 1970's and earlier chronology where humans and chimps split 14 Myr. That time scale has since been discarded. JOHN H. LANGDON email LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU DEPARTMENT OF BIOLOGY FAX (317) 788-3569 UNIVERSITY OF INDIANAPOLIS PHONE (317) 788-3447 INDIANAPOLIS, IN 46227 _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:93>From LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU Mon Oct 18 11:13:01 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 1993 11:13:01 -0500 From: "JOHN LANGDON" <LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: manuscript polymorphism In message <01H45A4IQD8E8Y5338@psc.plymouth.edu> writes: > Another idea crossed my mind, not one necessarily related to manuscript > polymorphism, but perhaps related to how some gene sequences remain un > changed for long periods of time. The example was published someplace > sometime ago (I can not remember where or when). It relates to the fact > that in General Biology textbooks, when the evolution of the horse is > described, the textbook authors state that Eohippus, one of the ancestral > forms, was the size of a (an I may have this somewhat wrong) collier/ > terrier, a dog from the coal mines of Wales. What is interesting, is the > fact that this dog is no longer a very common breed of dog, yet the > textbook writers rather than mutating the dog into a modern day form of > dog, eg, golden retriever, poodle, etc., continue to use the old name > as referenced in earlier textbooks. This example comes from one of Stephen Jay Gould's Natural History columns, and represents a theme he has repeated concerning the transmission and transmutation of myths and metaphors in the history of science. See "The Case of the Creeping Fox Terrier Clone", Natural History, January, 1988 pp. 16-24. JOHN H. LANGDON email LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU DEPARTMENT OF BIOLOGY FAX (317) 788-3569 UNIVERSITY OF INDIANAPOLIS PHONE (317) 788-3447 INDIANAPOLIS, IN 46227 _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:94>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Mon Oct 18 11:29:41 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 1993 12:36:29 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Clarification re: RFD: sci.evolution.human To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro Just a quick note of clarification on "RFD: sci.evolution.human". Danny Yee's note on sci.evolution.human (which was perfectly welcome here) was a forwarded copy of a formal "Request for Discussion" he had posted to the USENET discussion group "news.groups". "news.groups" is a discussion group on the USENET network that exists specifically for the purpose of discussing proposals of new USENET groups, and for formally voting on whether such groups should be established. A "Request for Discussion" is a formal document posted to news.groups, and USENET has a whole administrative structure and set of procedures for this sort of thing. The requested discussions are meant to take place on news.groups itself, and the only postings that have any effect on the process are those that are made to the news.groups section of USENET. The USENET administrative structure is something completely independent of listserv and similar mailing lists. I realize that many people here don't have access to USENET and may not be familiar with this procedure, so I thought this note of clarification might be helpful. If you are interested in the sci.evolution.human newsgroup and don't have USENET access you might want to get in touch with Danny Yee directly (danny@cs.su.oz.au), or ask your local computer center for a connection. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:95>From anemone@uno.cc.geneseo.edu Mon Oct 18 12:09:26 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 1993 13:12 EST From: anemone@uno.cc.geneseo.edu (Robert L. Anemone) Subject: RE: waterbabes & hominoid cladistics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Barry Roth recently posted this (edited) message and query: I do wonder how proponents of the aquatic ape scenario would respond to the following: >According to the DNA clock (and the figure on about p. 20 of _The Third >Chimpanzee_ by Jared Diamond), the split between the gorilla clade and >the clade (Homo,(chimpanzee,bonobo)) is set at about 1.7-2.0 Ma. So how >could climatic events in the late Miocene (ca. 7-5 Ma) have anything to >do with autapomorphies of Homo? I'm certain that I'm not the only Biological Anthropologist who noticed the obvious mistake in the date for the split between gorilla and human-chimp clades purported to come from Diamond's work. A quick look at the text of "The Third Chimpanzee" revealed the mistake to be Roth's, not Diamond's. The phylogenetic tree presented by Diamond as his Figure 1 (p. 21) has two different Y axes. The Y axis on the right side of the tree has units of millions of years ago (mya) while that on the left has units of percentage difference in DNA (%DNA). Roth mistakenly read the figure of "about 1.7-2.0 Ma" off the left axis (%DNA). The phylogenetic tree actually suggests that the percentage difference in DNA between gorilla and human-chimp clades is on the order of 2%, and that the time of divergence of these two clades was on the order of 10 mya. In his Figure legend, Diamond states: "...the common and pygmy chimps differ in about 0.7 percent of their DNA and diverged around three million years ago; we differ in 1.6 pecent of our DNA from either chimp and diverged from their common ancestor around seven million years ago; and gorillas differ in about 2.3 percent of their DNA from us or chimps and diverged from the common ancestor leading to us and the two chimps around ten million years ago." Please don't misconstrue my intent as providing some support for the "Aquatic Ape" Hypothesis. However, the split between humans and some great apes may very well have occurred during the Late Miocene. Bob Anemone Department of Anthropology SUNY at Geneseo _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:96>From SMITGM@hawkins.clark.edu Mon Oct 18 20:07:41 1993 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: "Gerard Donnelly Smith" <SMITGM@hawkins.clark.edu> Organization: Clark College, Vancouver WA, USA Date: 18 Oct 93 18:03:25 PST8PDT Subject: textual polymorphism An obnoxious myth that was only dismissed several years ago involved Galileo's discovery that the Earth revolved around the Sun. After 600 years, as you recall, the Catholic Church finally admitted he was correct and posthumously allowed him to be a Catholic again. Talk about over-representation of an error in later texts. It would be a curious study to see how long the Church's sanctioned science texts retained the error. Another study which might bear fruit would be an analysis of the changing rhetoric of creationism as it confronts more and more irrefutable evidence. Of course faith can move mountains, and even cause spatial and temporal distortions. I must say that any study of, or creation of, a theory of textual polymorphism (I'd like to move the discussion beyond manuscripts), should include ideology and its role in both substantive and non-substantive emendations in the text. Scientific texts, history texts, religious texts (obviously) arise from and conform to the ideologies surrounding them. While I am not sure which part of the genetic process to compare ideologies with, I would imagine the DNA represents the cultural code of which I speak. Errors or changes in locus, though a minor adjustment to the overall paradigm, are interesting phenomenon; however, those major adjustments in the "logos" of the text are more analogous to evolutionary changes or mutations we might see over several generations of a species being genetically manipulated. Discussing manuscript errors, though an interesting analogy, can not move beyond analogy and into possible application. Unless I am missing something, errors in transcription or even changes in single words or rhymes usually do not change the meaning of texts all that much. However, errors which occur in translating one language to the next can create quite significant errors. When these errors are purposeful, as in censorship or disinformation, they represent a genetic attempt by the cultural DNA to adjust to environmental imperatives and contraints. So, the cultural/ideological DNA of the Catholic Church ensured through the process called inquisitional censorship that the peasants of the 12th and 13th centuries did not experience the evolutionary shift in the paradigm until centuries after the discovery. We might call these peasants and priests, cultural or ideological Neanderthal's who could not, or were not allowed to adapt to change, ergo extinction. How might we apply this to current models of cultrual/ideological transmission? How might "survival of the fitest" be used to discuss cultrual transmission through texts? What scientific myths refuse to die, because there are just enough chromosomes encoded with that informations still out there in our mental soup? I see I've rambled too long. Dr. Gerard Donnelly-Smith e-mail: smitgm@hawkins.clark.edu English Department phone: 206-699-0478 Clark College Vancouver, WA 98663 _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:97>From CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU Mon Oct 18 20:53:33 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 93 13:41:10 BS3 From: charbel nino el-hani <CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: a reference in altruism To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Here is the reference i forgot in my first e-mail to darwin-l and one more article related to the debate on altruism: Nowak, M. A. & May, R. M. 1992. NATUre, vol. 359:826-829 (see also the news and views, by karl sigmund, *on prisoners and cells*, on page 774. nowak, m. a. & May, r. m. 1993. the spatial dilemmas of evolution. international journal of bifurcation and chaos, vol. 3, no. 1:35-78. Charbel nino el-hani msc in education/institute of biology federal university of bahia, salvador, bahia, brazil research area: historical epistemology address: charbel@brufba _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:98>From WILLS@macc.wisc.edu Mon Oct 18 21:55:21 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 93 21:58 CDT From: Jeffrey Wills <WILLS@macc.wisc.edu> Subject: mauscripts and genetics--Why care? To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu [First: I missed a day of Darwin (by digest), so I apologize if I overlap with another poster.] A few contributors have politely asked what the point is of pushing the manuscript/genetics analogy so far. Intellectual ambits can always be widened, but hasn't this analogy gone far enough? A year ago I would have agreed, but I was very impressed with the results (not just theory) of the O'Hara-Robinson experiment last year. As several of you know, with the use of a cladistics program O'Hara was able in a matter of minutes to take a binary data set of manuscript variants and produce a stemma (a historical tree of the manuscripts) which was fairly accurate (as judged against Robinson's lengthy work by hand). It is now quite clear to me that the evolutionary biologists have some handy devices which we can use in stemmatics, but we need to know more about them to learn how to improve their accuracy or adapt them to our needs. The fruits of detailed discussions about ms./genetic transmission processes and selections will be in our improved use of such programs--at the very least. What do biologists have to gain from the conversation? Someone else would have to speak to that. Since there are many more biologists with much more research money, my suspicion is that the benefits are decidedly lopsided toward the manuscript side. On the other hand, the nature of the linguistic or manuscript record has some comparative advantages. Occasionally for manuscripts there is external data that lets us know where they were, who copied them and when. This allows us an external check on our method of reconstruction. This evidence about specific mutations between items in our manuscript trees is not really availabe for the fossil record, I suspect. It is not just more fossil evidence, it is a different type of evidence. Likewise the linguistic record has many problems, but sometimes we have enough material to go back more closely and confirm or reject a reconstruction of dialect spread, for example. Jeffrey Wills wills@macc.wisc.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:99>From WILLS@macc.wisc.edu Mon Oct 18 22:14:03 1993 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 93 22:15 CDT From: Jeffrey Wills <WILLS@macc.wisc.edu> Subject: manuscripts, populations, horizontal transmission To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Bob O'Hara wrote recently: "There are many differences between manuscript transmission and both linguistic and biological evolution, most notably that manuscript transmission is not a populational phenomenon (it is closer to being clonal, with a fair bit of horizontal transmission), but I will leave those issues for a later discussion so as to emphasize the particular issue of ploidy/polymorphism." Perhaps "The time has come, the Walrus said . . ." to speak of population phenomena and horizontal transmission. Let me ask what "population phenomena" and "horizontal transmission" mean? I assume population refers to the fact that species or languages on a tree represent group phenomena whereas manuscript trees have discrete single entities as their "individuals" on the tree. Although this is usually true, in large manuscript lineages we do refer to clusters of similar manuscripts as families and occasionally treat the group as a single entity. On those occasions however we usually start to feel there is too much contamination to even draw a tree so we don't. As a result most of the manuscript trees you will see are of closed traditions and tidy data sets. This tends to bias the way we represent the manuscript history. Also, unlike biologists who are interested in the whole tree, paleographers are usually interested just in the best witnesses and later descendants are less studied and often not represented on trees at all. This may blind us to issues we would see if we studied the whole tree. The other term (horizontal transmission) is a familiar term in the world of manuscripts and is used to describe the influence of a manuscript (s) other than the parent manuscript on a copy. Does the term have a separate meaning in biology? The suggestion that manuscript copying is clonal makes good sense: one manuscript goes in, the same manuscript and another one come out. But as Bob points out there is also coded material which is not from the source code and is not random. How can something be part clonal and part not? This may seem bizarre, but I think we need to put the scribes into the tree too. It's a terribly uneven sort of hybridization, but I think it has some explanatory value. Example 1: I am copying along a passage in Virgil's Aeneid which goes "de nomine Phoebi" (by the name of Apollo) and I write "de nomine Christi" (by the name of Christ). This is not a mutation like transposing letters or omitting a word, in which the scribe can be seen as a faulty mechanic. In the case of "de nomine Christi", the scribe has entered new code into the tradition which is nowhere else attested in any manuscript of the Aeneid. Example 2: I am copying the copy generated above, which reads "de nomine Christi". I know this is nonsense; the passage is talking about the temple of Apollo; Virgil died before Christ, I think I remember the line, etc. so I change this to "de nomine Phoebi". Where did this "new" reading come from? It was not in the text in front of me, and it is not random; in fact it is ancestral. I must be carrying another copy of the text inside me. The reality is that texts are recorded in memory as well as in writing. Potentially each scribe has a polyploid version of the text in memory. Is it impossible to consider each copying of a manuscript not as a cloning but as a mating between the scribe's memory and the manuscript at hand? Although of different natures apparently, both have a copy of something in the same code. In most instances, the manuscript is so dominant and the scribe so recessive that the issue of this mating looks almost entirely like the manuscript. But when I copy a manuscript some of that text is also copied into my memory. When I next copy a manuscript of the same text, the physical manuscript may not be so dominant and my internal version may not be so recessive. Or if I come across a hole (lacuna) in the manuscript, my linguistic resources will be dominant. Traditionally, paleographers (scholars of manuscripts) see the tree as constantly branching (a closed tradition) and are very embarrassed/confused by any effort to connect nodes on the tree (called "contamination" or "horizontal transmission"). Yet how does the code leap across from branch to branch if the mutation process is strictly clonal? I am very innocent of what "clonal" means to a biologist, but I think it is the great mistake of our manuscript trees (and our language trees, but that is a topic for another day) to assume they are clonal. It boxes us into a process which is inherently faulty. It is true that once we say manuscripts intermate with memories we have really grafted the manuscript trees onto the language tree and that is a mess. But I think it is what happens. In the past decade, for various literary projects thousands of megabytes of text have been entered in machine readable-form. Experience has shown that scanning is not good enough (because the cost of error-correction is too high) and double manual entry (keyboard entry by two separate people whose results are then matched for differences) offers the best results by price. It is also clear that the less the keyboarders know the language the better (we have entered hundreds of megabytes of Greek texts through companies in the Phillipines or Singapore). Accidental human error in copying always exists, but those sources of error must be distinguished theoretically from "intelligent" error. Possibly there is no biological counterpart for this intelligent error, but I think it is given some account by a view at mating rather than cloning. Jeffrey Wills wills@macc.wisc.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:100>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Tue Oct 19 00:30:18 1993 Date: Tue, 19 Oct 1993 01:37:16 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: October 19 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro OCTOBER 19 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1605/1682: Sir THOMAS BROWNE, antiquary, author, and sometime physician, born on this day in 1605 in St. Michael's Parish, Cheapside, London. He will die on the same date in 1682. After education at Oxford and travel on the Continent he will settle in Norwich, England. The discovery of several ancient burial urns in Norfolk will lead Browne to think about reconstructing aspects of the past that are not recorded in textual sources, and he will express these thoughts in one of the most graceful and imaginative documents of the antiquarian period, his _Hydriotaphia, or Urne-Buriall_ (1658): "What Song the Syrens sang, or what name Achilles assumed when he hid himself among women, though puzling Questions, are not beyond all conjecture. What time the persons of these Ossuaries entered the famous Nations of the dead, and slept with Princes and Counsellours, might admit a wide solution." Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc. ukans.edu, a network discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. E-mail darwin@iris.uncg.edu for more information. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:101>From 72350.1764@CompuServe.COM Tue Oct 19 13:40:47 1993 Date: 19 Oct 93 14:31:29 EDT From: Earth Magazine <72350.1764@CompuServe.COM> To: charbel nino el-hani <CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: Re: a reference in altruism I believe that there was another News and Views on Nowak and May's work in Nature last summer. It reported that Nowak and May found that if they make the cells in their grid fallible -- that is made them sometimes think that the other cells had defected when in fact they had cooperated -- another strategy did better than Tit For Tat. I think they call the new strategy Win Stay Lose Switch. But even with fallibility was still possible for a cooperative strategy to spread through the grid. Tom Waters _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:102>From hantuo@utu.fi Tue Oct 19 17:43:12 1993 To: Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: hantuo@utu.fi (Hanna Tuomisto) Subject: water babies Date: Wed, 20 Oct 1993 00:46:41 +0200 Iain Davidson wrote: >Elaine Morgan raises some really interesting problems for which her >elaboration of the theory seem to offer a plauible explanation, but >plausibility is not really enough. Maybe, but as long as the alternatives are much less plausible, Morgan's explanations remain (in my opinion) a better choice for a working hypotheses. P.E. Griffiths wrote: >Morgan's argument for the aquatic ape hypothesis is typical of a class of >adaptationist arguments which try to increase the plausibility of an >hypothesised adaptive phase by listing a large number of traits which it >can simultaneously explain. It does this quite impressively. >Bob O'Hara (1988) has drawn attention to the dangers of giving adaptive >explanations of character states without paying attention to the cladistic >relationships of those states. >In this particular case, the argument falls down unless the proposed >'adaptive character suite' emerges in the same general area of the tree for >primate lineages. If, instead, it is a collage of traits from different >portions of the tree then it cannot be a response to a single adaptive >phase. All the traits that are discussed by Morgan are apomorphies of Homo sapiens. At least they are not shared with any other known primate species, present or extinct, with the exception of those that are supposed to be our immediate ancestors. When non-primate mammals (or birds, for that matter) are considered, the "human" 'adaptive character suite' appears in several non-related species, and then it must obviously be the result of convergent evolution: adaptation of separate evolutionary lineages to shared environmental conditions. It just so happens that all these species live in aquatic environments. JOHN H. LANGDON wrote: >There is still enough uncertainty in this date that the strict chronology does >not rule out an extremely (phenomonally) rapid aquatic adaptation, but one >that effectively discards Australopithecus. However, the aquatic ape model >(proposed 1960, developed 1972) makes the most sense under the 1970's and >earlier chronology where humans and chimps split 14 Myr. That time scale has >since been discarded. The evolution of humans may indeed be characterized as extremely rapid. Now why did humans evolve more rapidly than the other apes? Usually rapid evolution is supposed to be connected with rapid environmental changes that impose new selection pressures to a species, and that is the standard explanation of the savanna hypothesis, too. However, the forest-savanna boundary does not seem like a drastical enough environmental change to provoke so fundamental morphological and physiological changes as humans have, especially since these traits make no sense in the savanna environment. An (semi)aquatic phase in human history explains nicely both the rapidity of the evolutionary change and the peculiarly marine character of those traits that distinguish us from other primates. Hanna Tuomisto Department of Biology University of Turku FIN-20500 Turku, FINLAND Phone +358-21-6335634 Fax +358-21-6335564 e-mail hantuo@utu.fi _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:103>From D.Oldroyd@unsw.edu.au Tue Oct 19 18:01:32 1993 Date: Wed, 20 Oct 1993 09:11:59 +1000 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: D.Oldroyd@unsw.edu.au Subject: Nowak and May reference/Tom Walters Dear Tom Walters, I was interested to see your brief note about tit for tat strategies and a reference to something in Nature. One of my students has, I suspect, produced a somewhat similar result as a result of his computer simulation of the prisoners' dilemma. He has done this more or less for his own amusement, rather than as a research project. I am not an expert on such matters. Could you, or any other contributor to Darwin-l, kindly provide me with the reference to the Nature article and the Nowak and May reference? We would find it interesting, I'm sure. Thank you in anticipation, Sincerely, David Oldroyd, University of New South Wales D.Oldroyd@unsw.EDU.AU David Oldroyd, School of Science and Technology Studies, University of New South Wales _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:104>From LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU Wed Oct 20 10:45:45 1993 Date: Wed, 20 Oct 1993 10:45:45 -0500 From: "JOHN LANGDON" <LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: water babies In message <93Oct20.004643eet.144261-4@utu.fi> writes: > Iain Davidson wrote: > >Elaine Morgan raises some really interesting problems for which her > >elaboration of the theory seem to offer a plauible explanation, but > >plausibility is not really enough. > > Maybe, but as long as the alternatives are much less plausible, Morgan's > explanations remain (in my opinion) a better choice for a working > hypotheses. Why are the alternatives much less plausible? Are you familiar with them? > All the traits that are discussed by Morgan are apomorphies of Homo > sapiens. At least they are not shared with any other known primate species, > present or extinct, with the exception of those that are supposed to be our > immediate ancestors. When non-primate mammals (or birds, for that matter) > are considered, the "human" 'adaptive character suite' appears in several > non-related species, and then it must obviously be the result of convergent > evolution: adaptation of separate evolutionary lineages to shared > environmental conditions. It just so happens that all these species live in > aquatic environments. I would argue that this is not true. Some of the traits she cites, particularly aspects of reproduction which was a major thrust of her first book, are not apomorphies. More recent primatology has found that pair bonding, estrous/menstrual patterns, extended sexual receptivity (tolerance?), etc., do not distinguish humans from other animals. Many of the list of apomorphies belong to single adaptive complexes that require only a single explanation. Rather than considering them independently as evidence, they should be lumped. Evolutionary convergence does not necessarily imply adaptation to the same environmental conditions. Convergence with other species, especially distantly related ones should be examined carefully and in phylogenetic as well as functional context. For example, reduction of hair, increase in fat, increase in sweating, changes in cutaneous innervation and circulation all relate to a thermoregulation strategy. While some of these are paralleled in aquatic animals, others (sweating, circulation) do not make sense as aquatic adaptations and are not paralleled in aquatic animals. Replacement of hair with fat was a convergence for humans, pigs, and whales as a more efficient solution to heat loss. Whales are aquatic. Should we assume that pigs are too? Humans and pigs are terrestrial and show it by their concern with heat dissipation as well-- pigs wallow and humans flush and sweat. Why are whales a better model for humans than pigs? Bipedalism is not much of an adaptation for aquatic life. Seals and otters are not bipedal. Standing in the water allows you to exploit water up to five feet or so in height. (For Lucy, maybe up to four feet.) If you really wanted to collect shell fish from water this shallow, wait until the tide goes out like clam rakers do. If you are going to swim in deeper water, why be bipedal. On the other hand, if you are going to become bipedal, then you may be secondarily adapted for swimming (as we are), because both utilize powerful lower limbs. Are we really well adapted to the water? What other aquatic species looses as many of its members in drowning accidents? > JOHN H. LANGDON wrote: > >There is still enough uncertainty in this date that the strict chronology > >does not rule out an extremely (phenomonally) rapid aquatic adaptation, but > >one that effectively discards Australopithecus. However, the aquatic ape > >model (proposed 1960, developed 1972) makes the most sense under the 1970's > >and earlier chronology where humans and chimps split 14 Myr. That time scale > >has since been discarded. > > The evolution of humans may indeed be characterized as extremely rapid. Now > why did humans evolve more rapidly than the other apes? Usually rapid > evolution is supposed to be connected with rapid environmental changes that > impose new selection pressures to a species, and that is the standard > explanation of the savanna hypothesis, too. However, the forest-savanna > boundary does not seem like a drastical enough environmental change to > provoke so fundamental morphological and physiological changes as humans > have, especially since these traits make no sense in the savanna > environment. An (semi)aquatic phase in human history explains nicely both > the rapidity of the evolutionary change and the peculiarly marine character > of those traits that distinguish us from other primates. What I meant in this comment was that if we had to place the aquatic adaptation AND its reversal in this narrower time frame (between 7 and 5 Myr), then this would be impossibly rapid. There was indeed a climatic change in eastern Africa at the end of the Miocene and into the Pliocene. That very likely correlates with speciation and rapid evolution of our lineage in the conventional models for terrestrial evolution. Evolutionary mechanisms, not chronology, make the aquatic diversion unlikely. If this critique of the aquatic hypothesis is spotty, it is because there are so many angles to attack that I hardly know where to begin-- evolutionary improbability, the plausibility of conventional models, the lack of parsimony of Morgan's model, errors in it (some only apparent in light of more recent knowledge). I made a similar reply on the anthro-l list recently. I append it below to flesh out some of these points. From: "JOHN LANGDON" <LANGDON> Date: Fri, Oct 8, 1993 1:09 PM To: dhanson@osteon.win.net ANTHRO-L@UBVM.cc.buffalo.edu Subject: Re: The Aquatic Ape - news (fwd); replies to Douglas Adams please As a paleoanthropologist, I cannot let this discussion pass by without comment. The first questions I would ask in critiquing the aquatic theory are: What does the aquatic theory explain better that the more conventional (terrestrial) models? Can the individual hypotheses of the aquatic theory be better explained by conventional models? What other predictions/expectations are to be derived from the aquatic theory? Can these be used to test the theory? Is it reasonable in light of our larger understanding of primate and human evolution and of evolutionary theory? I suspect that it has been dismissed with little published examination largely because it fails these questions. > His guess was that the reason it was not taken seriously was that most of the > people working in this field were used to looking to the fossil record for > evidence, and there simply isn't any - but then it is fairly astonishing that > we have any evidence for anything in the fossil record. Don't sell the fossil record short. Blanket statements such as this are commonly found in creationist literature. The fossil record can tell us quite a bit. For example, the skeletal anatomy of Australopithecus from 3.0 myr is reasonably well known, even if we are not in agreement on how to interpret it. The highly fragmentary fossils earlier than that indicate the presence of Australopithecus afarensis (or at least its teeth) in inland habitats a couple of million years previously. This narrows the temporal window for the aquatic phase and its reversal. > For instance: EM makes much of the fact that man, uniquely amongst > terrestrial mammals, is bipedal. She argues that there is a very great > difference between an anatomical structure which is essentially quadrapedal > but allows for bipedal locomotion on occasions, and an anatomical structure > that is exclusively bipedal. She maintains that there is no argument in > favour of an animal making the huge anatomical changes and sacrifices > necessary that wouldn't apply equally to many other animals, none of which > have made that change. (The argument which says that man, uniquely, became > bipedal in order to carry tools implies that man intended to become > toolmakers which is obviously ridiculous, but nevertheless seems to slip past > people's defences with surprising ease.) [Incidentally, since her first book presented the model in a rather strident feminist rhetoric, I presume that Morgan would prefer gender-neutral terminology. Humans, not just men, are bipedal.] I am in agreement with her criticism of the cultural models for bipedalism. One cannot comfortably use arguments to explain unique human traits that apply just as easily to other species. An explanation for human bipedalism must build on the unique context of our ancestors before bipedalism as we recognize it evolved, not afterwards. Cannot the same criticism be leveled at the aquatic hypothesis? Why are there no other bipedal aquatic tetrapods (aside from birds, which don't count). Humans can and do exploit coastal resources, but I doubt they could have survived just on those that lie in water shallow enough for us to stand up and breathe. One would expect that humans adapted for the water would have been as reliant on swimming as muskrats and otters. I don't find this a convincing reason for becoming bipedal. > I looked up bipedalism in the CEHE, but while it went into the mechanism of > bipedalism in some detail, it passed over the question of how it could > possibly have arisen rather briefly and vaguely (p79), which surprised me. > Isn't the evolution of a completely unique and expensive feature like > bipedalism rather significant? I think it is significant. I don't want to speak for the CEHE, which I have not read, but I believe bipedalism to have been one of the first and most fundamental adaptive shifts which distinguishes the hominid lineage. Is there a better explanation for it? I think so. As several anthropologists (including myself) have argued in print, the alternative to human bipedalism is not the efficient quadrupedalism of most mammals, but the specialized arboreal climbing anatomy of modern apes, who are not spectacularly efficient on the ground. A climbing ape committing itself to terrestrialism, would do well to make whatever anatomical adjustments were necessary to increase its locomotor efficiency. Good monkey-like quadrupedalism is one possibility, but given the tendency for upright posture and substantial hindlimb weight-bearing, bipedalism was an equally viable strategy. Why did our ancestors "choose" bipedalism? Many authors have argued that it was because of cultural behaviors (e.g. food carrying) that we preferred that alternative. [This argument is far different from the simplistic model that says we left an efficient, well-adapted quadrupedal posture for cultural reasons.] I prefer to consider the possibility that we took the bipedal alternative by fortuitous chance. > >EM also makes much of our hairlessness, also an almost unique feature > amongst terrestrial mammals. How did that arise? I looked in the CEHE but > there was no entry for 'hair' in the index. I would have thought that the > evolutionary signicance of hair in the only terrestrial mammal to have > dispensed with it was worth a mention. So I tried to track the subject down > by following various other leads from EM's chapter on hair. > For instance she talks at length about the relative functions of eccrine and > apocrine glands in other animals and in ourselves. She says that whereas we > might expected to have apocrine scent and sweat glands all over our bodies > like our ancestors, ours have atrophied and are only found in limited areas > of the body. Instead we now sweat dilute salt water from modified eccrine > glands which other animals use to secret moisture on to their paws to get a > good grip. Whatever the reason for this change, (and EM, of course, argues > that it flows directly from our alleged aquatic period) something must have > caused it. If not an aquatic period, then what? I looked up eccrine and > apocrine glands in the CEHE and found almost nothing, certainly no sense that > there was anything here that needed to be investigated or explained. Is EM > right that our arrangement is highly unusual? If it is highly unusual isn't > that significant? If it is significant, then what is wrong with the > consequent arguments made by EM? Maybe a lot - I just can't find out what. > >The CEHE mentions eccrine glands in the entry about sweat on page 48. It > >says that our sweating mechanism is particularly effective because it pours > >copious quantities of weakly saline water on to our skin to cool us down. > >This means of course that we have to pour copious quantities of water back > >into ourselves. It doesn't make clear why this profligate expenditure of > >moisture (which often drips off us before it has a chance to cool us by > >evaporation) is better for us than the more economical systems used by other > >terrestrial mammals. The entry states that 'our lack of body hair also > >ensures that sweat provides very efficient cooling as it evaporates from the > >heated skin.' If this is true, then why is it that every other terrestrial > >mammal uses fur as an insulator against both heat and cold, while humans > >have to wear clothes to cope with either? I wouldn't like to have to wander > >round the savannah during the day protected only by my sweat glands. Or at > >night, for that matter. There is no entry in the index for 'clothes'. EM > >also says that human skin is unusual in having an abundance of subcutaneous > >fat, which is characteristic of marine rather than terrestrial mammals. From > >all this she draws a variety of conclusions which support her thesis in a > >pretty logical and straightforward way. P49 of the CEHE makes the only > >mention of subcutaneous fat, but doesn't say whether we have more or less of > >it than other mammals. It notes that 'Fat is particularly beneficial in cold > >water, because neither fur nor clothing provides significant insulation in > >these conditions.' It doesn't follow this line of thought anywhere, though, > >other than to say that successful Channel swimmers are usually the fatter > >ones. She is right in describing modern human anatomy. The characteristics she observed make a consistent picture: Humans are adapted for sustained high levels of energy expenditure, particularly in hot environments. Sweating permits us to cool down more efficiently; it also serves as a supplement to the waste-filtering system of the kidneys. Relying on fat for insulation rather than hair permits us to bypass that insulation with the bloodstream when necessary, so that excess heat can be dumped through the skin. Typical furry mammals in hot climates use their insulation as much to keep body heat in as to keep solar heat out. This means they have a narrower range of thermal tolerance and thus a limited ability to shift to high metabolic gear for very long. Louis Leakey demonstrated this by running some fast savannah species to exhaustion. Aquatic species (e.g. hippos) have pursued a strategy resembling ours for a different reason: water generally penetrates fur and neutralizes its ability to insulate. Thus fat is better for them. Clothes were undoubtedly a more recent cultural invention to cope with climates outside of the tropics. (Even Morgan would have to concede that our ancestors did not put on bathrobes as soon as they came out of the water. Yes, there are limits to running around in the tropical sun. People generally don't-- only mad dogs and Englishmen, as the saying goes. To me, human skin is reasonably explained. I am investigating parallel adaptations in other body systems. The aquatic theory offers nothing in this line that we don't already have. > So is it the idea that an animal might have been forced into an aquatic > environment, started to adapt, and was then forced back into a land > environment? I happen to find this a very unlikely scenario, given the very narrow time period allowed for it. > >The underlying assumption is that the place at which we have arrived is the > >one towards which we have always been inexorably heading. To think anything > >else makes us feel insecure of our very existence. > >The Aquatic Ape Theory, by contrast, gives us picture of a world which > >happened to tip a few of us into the water and then happened to tip us back > >out again when we were half-done. I find the aquatic hypothesis as guilty of this teological thinking as Darwin. It focuses entirely on modern anatomy, asking how we got here and ignores the really essential consideration of any evolutionary explanation: What did we start with and how did that influence our pathway? Nor does she really make any use of what we know of the fossil record. The fossils give no hint of any such aquatic diversion in the transition from semi-arboreal apes to terrestrial humans. Morgan's other main body of argument comes from reproduction. It also offers no improvement on conventional models and incorporates some of the same problems. In short, I fail to see where the aquatic theory has anything to offer that is better than more conventional ones, and there is no direct evidence in support of it. She says swimming predisposed us to bipedalism. Isn't it equally likely that bipedalism predisposed us to swimming? All of this is not disproof, but it argues that the aquatic hypothesis is superfluous and unparsimonious. I suggest this is a major reason anthropologists have generally ignored it. I could go on, but I am out of time. My class started nine minutes ago. JOHN H. LANGDON email LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU DEPARTMENT OF BIOLOGY FAX (317) 788-3569 UNIVERSITY OF INDIANAPOLIS PHONE (317) 788-3447 INDIANAPOLIS, IN 46227 _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:105>From 72350.1764@CompuServe.COM Wed Oct 20 12:08:26 1993 Date: 20 Oct 93 12:56:24 EDT From: Earth Magazine <72350.1764@CompuServe.COM> To: "INTERNET:D.Oldroyd@unsw.edu.au" <D.Oldroyd@unsw.edu.au> Subject: Re: Nowak and May reference David Oldroyd, The News and Views was "Cooperation wins and stays," by Manfred Milinski, Vol. 364, page 12. The issue was dated July 1, 1993. It discussed a Nowak and May paper, "A strategy of win-stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the Prisoner's Dilemma game," Vol. 364, page 56. The latest News Scientist (Oct. 9) has a news item on a criticism of May and Nowak's work made by Robert Huberman of Xerox PARC. Huberman says that M&N's model only works if all of the cells in the grid revise their strategies simultaneously. M&N say this isn't so. Huberman apparently has a paper in PNAS, vol 90, page 7716. I'm interested in hearing whether anyone thinks that May and Nowak's work is consonant with Kropotkin's views as expressed in "Mutual Aid," a book which sits I'm afraid unread on my shelf. (By the way, Kropotkin was well known for his work on glacial history and physical geography before he became a revolutionary. He was born Dec 21, 1842, a potential entry for "Today In Historical Sciences.") Tom Waters _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:106>From princeh@husc.harvard.edu Wed Oct 20 14:27:32 1993 Date: Wed, 20 Oct 1993 15:05:21 -0400 (EDT) From: Patricia Princehouse <princeh@husc.harvard.edu> Subject: Re: mss polymorphism/dog breeds To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu On Mon, 18 Oct 1993 LARRYS@psc.plymouth.edu wrote: > that in General Biology textbooks, when the evolution of the horse is > described, the textbook authors state that Eohippus, one of the ancestral > forms, was the size of a (an I may have this somewhat wrong) collier/ > terrier, a dog from the coal mines of Wales. What is interesting, is the > fact that this dog is no longer a very common breed of dog, yet the > textbook writers rather than mutating the dog into a modern day form of > dog, eg, golden retriever, poodle, etc., continue to use the old name > as referenced in earlier textbooks. This may stray a bit too far from the subject, but as a dog fancier, I would be very interested in any reference you could provide as to this collier/terrier breed used in coal mines. The only reference I know on the textbook subject is Gould's Nat Hist column from @ 1987 which shows the change going from fox (as in Vulpes) to Fox Terrier (a breed still very much around today) and continuing as Fox Terrier in generations of text books. I'm familiar with many rare terriers but have never heard of one from Welsh coal mines. If you can think of any possible reference at all, I'd be eager to hear of it. (by the way, there's nothing new about the Golden Retriever and Poodle. Like the Fox Terrier, these breeds have been around much longer than the taxon Eohippus or even Hyracotherium). Thanks, -Patricia Princehouse _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:107>From hantuo@utu.fi Wed Oct 20 15:02:49 1993 To: Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: hantuo@utu.fi (Hanna Tuomisto) Subject: water babies Date: Wed, 20 Oct 1993 22:05:55 +0200 >Why are the alternatives much less plausible? Are you familiar with them? Yes, I'm familiar with them, I've traced back quite a few of the original publications. Elaine Morgan has given an excellent answer to why they are less plausible in her book "The Scars of Evolution. What our bodies tell us about human origins". But I have not kept an eye on what has been published during the last two years, so if you (or anyone else) know of any new papers that are relevant to the topic, I'd be grateful to hear about them. >> All the traits that are discussed by Morgan are apomorphies of Homo >> sapiens. At least they are not shared with any other known primate species, ..(stuff deleted) >I would argue that this is not true. Some of the traits she cites, >particularly aspects of reproduction which was a major thrust of her first >book, are not apomorphies. More recent primatology has found that pair >bonding, estrous/menstrual patterns, extended sexual receptivity (tolerance?), >etc., do not distinguish humans from other animals. Sorry for my sloppy usage of terminology here. I was thinking of those traits Morgan discusses in The Aquatic Ape and in The Scars of Evolution. Her first book, The Descent of Woman, was not written to advocate the aquatic theory. It was written to draw attention to the fact that at that time (sixties-seventies) human evolution was written about as if it were just male evolution, with females passively following along. It's in the two last books were she elaborated the aquatic ape theory, and that's where the sharpest arguments are found. I would like it to be otherwise, but hers are the only texts I've seen so far that really weight the different theories on the basis of their scientific merits. >Many of the list of apomorphies belong to single adaptive complexes that >require only a single explanation. Rather than considering them independently >as evidence, they should be lumped. You said it! This precisely is a very strong argument in favor of the aquatic hypothesis. Put all the characteristics together that are apomorphies in humans, and what do you get? The aquatic syndrome, many traits of which are known from both birds and mammals. >Evolutionary convergence does not necessarily imply adaptation to the same >environmental conditions. Convergence with other species, especially distantly >related ones should be examined carefully and in phylogenetic as well as >functional context. Sure, but if phylogeny does not give the answers, you're left with the environmental explanation. >For example, reduction of hair, increase in fat, increase in sweating, changes >in cutaneous innervation and circulation all relate to a thermoregulation >strategy. While some of these are paralleled in aquatic animals, others >(sweating, circulation) do not make sense as aquatic adaptations and are not >paralleled in aquatic animals. Humans are not aquatic now, and no one has suggested that they have ever been fully aquatic. Obviously sweating makes no sense for a fully aquatic mammal, because there's no evaporation when you're submerged. On the contrary, there are lots of land mammals that cool themselves by sweating (e.g. horses), but the difference between them and ourselves is that they don't waste salts and water. When humans sweat, most of the water does not even evaporate, it drips off. Someone living close to a permanent source of salt and water, like a seashore, could afford that kind of a system, but both substances are difficult to find in the savanna. The only savanna mammals to my knowledge that sweat as copiously as humans are the hippos, and they leave the water only at night. >Replacement of hair with fat was a convergence >for humans, pigs, and whales as a more efficient solution to heat loss. Whales >are aquatic. Should we assume that pigs are too? No. >Humans and pigs are terrestrial >and show it by their concern with heat dissipation as well-- pigs wallow and >humans flush and sweat. Why are whales a better model for humans than pigs? Whales are NOT a better model. Both animals show equally well the thermoregulation point: when a mammal lives in an environment that keeps it wet, it gets rid of its hair and develops subcutaneous fat instead. Whales are fully aquatic, humans are not. Pigs are mud-dwellers, humans are not. >Bipedalism is not much of an adaptation for aquatic life. Seals and otters are >not bipedal. They did not evolve from primates either, so their bodies did not have the flexibility that enabled our ancestors to stand up. >Standing in the water allows you to exploit water up to five feet >or so in height. (For Lucy, maybe up to four feet.) If you really wanted to >collect shell fish from water this shallow, wait until the tide goes out like >clam rakers do. You may want to go into the water to avoid terrestrial predators, or to cross a river. You need much less energy for wading if you have most of your body out of the water. There are two other primates who habitually wade in a bipedal position: the Japanese macaque and the proboscis monkey. >If you are going to swim in deeper water, why be bipedal. In deep water the important thing is not that you're bipedal, but that you're streamlined. The most streamlined position for a primate is to stretch the legs backwards parallelly to the spine. Retain that posture when you go to land, and you become bipedal. >On the other hand, if you are going to become bipedal, then you may be >secondarily adapted for swimming (as we are), because both utilize powerful >lower limbs. I'd say it went the other way round. Bipedalism is a very difficult way of walking and has necessitated impressive changes in our anatomy and physiology. With the support of water, that change could take place relatively painlessly. >Are we really well adapted to the water? What other aquatic species looses as >many of its members in drowning accidents? What other PRIMATE loses so many in drowning accidents? None, since they would not play in water like humans do. Except perhaps the Japanese macaques and the proboscis monkeys. >Evolutionary mechanisms, not chronology, make the >aquatic diversion unlikely. Why? >If this critique of the aquatic hypothesis is spotty, it is because there are >so many angles to attack that I hardly know where to begin-- evolutionary >improbability, the plausibility of conventional models, the lack of parsimony >of Morgan's model, errors in it (some only apparent in light of more recent >knowledge). If you know a more parsimonious theory than Morgan's, please tell me. All I've seen is a bunch of statements about single characteristics, most of them contradicting each other. I've never seen a coherent terrestrial theory. I am very interested in that recent knowledge that shows errors in the aquatic one. >Cannot the same criticism be leveled at the aquatic hypothesis? Why are there >no other bipedal aquatic tetrapods (aside from birds, which don't count). Why don't they count? The penguins walk on land in a remarkably similar position than humans do, in spite of the fact that non-aquatic birds have their spines horizontally when they walk. (I know that geese have their spines horizontally. But they don't dive, they float.) >Good monkey-like quadrupedalism is one possibility, but given the tendency for >upright posture and substantial hindlimb weight-bearing, bipedalism was an >equally viable strategy. Except that lifting the spine into a vertical position is a totally new construction for the entire mammalian lineage. Quadrupedalism could have been attained by a simple reversal to the original way of locomotion. >I find the aquatic hypothesis as guilty of this teological thinking as Darwin. >It focuses entirely on modern anatomy, asking how we got here and ignores the >really essential consideration of any evolutionary explanation: What did we >start with and how did that influence our pathway? Nor does she really make >any use of what we know of the fossil record. The fossils give no hint of any >such aquatic diversion in the transition from semi-arboreal apes to >terrestrial humans. Dear John Langdon: You cannot have read Morgan's books. She devotes entire chapters to the fossil record. We seem to be talking about entirely different theories. Let's make a deal: send out the references to the papers/books that describe The orthodox Terrestrial Theory, so I can learn what exactly you are defending. Then you read Morgan's books so you know what I'm defending. Otherwise we just keep arguing without getting anywhere. Hanna Tuomisto hantuo@utu.fi _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:108>From CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU Wed Oct 20 15:39:19 1993 Resent-Date: Wed, 20 Oct 93 18:36:37 BS3 Date: Mon, 18 Oct 93 13:41:10 BS3 Resent-From: Charbel Nino El-Hani <CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: a reference in altruism Resent-To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Here is the reference i forgot in my first e-mail to darwin-l and one more article related to the debate on altruism: Nowak, M. A. & May, R. M. 1992. NATUre, vol. 359:826-829 (see also the news and views, by karl sigmund, *on prisoners and cells*, on page 774. nowak, m. a. & May, r. m. 1993. the spatial dilemmas of evolution. international journal of bifurcation and chaos, vol. 3, no. 1:35-78. Charbel nino el-hani msc in education/institute of biology federal university of bahia, salvador, bahia, brazil research area: historical epistemology address: charbel@brufba _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:109>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Wed Oct 20 23:29:54 1993 Date: Thu, 21 Oct 1993 00:35:52 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: New book on statistics in archeology and history To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro This new book notice just appeared on ARCH-L, the archeology discussion group. I thought it might be of interest to some Darwin-L members also. Bob O'Hara darwin@iris.uncg.edu ------- Begin forwarded message ------- Version 2 (History and Archaeology) of Essentials of Statistical Methods by Dr. T. P. Hutchinson. There are many excellent introductory textbooks of statistics available. But so many of them are 500, 700, even 1000 pages long. This has real disadvantages. They are heavy. They are expensive. And they are wordy. But here is a volume light enough to be carried around, and cheap enough for every student to afford. A typical introductory statistics course gets as far as some techniques of inference -- the testing of hypotheses and the construction of confidence intervals. That is the subject of Part III of this book. In preparation for this, the student needs to know about data description and about probability. These are covered in Parts I and II. In this Version, many of the examples are taken from history and archaeology. For instance: in Part I, the percentages of lead in Bronze Age sickles, and the price of rice in 18th-century China; in Part II, the probability of throwing "Venus" with four astragali, and the occurrence of wars viewed as a Poisson process; in Part III, the sizes of shells in a possible Aboriginal midden, and drought causing the fall of Tiwanaku. Published September 1993. A5 format. Paperback. xii + 152 pages. Index of 800 entries. Price: $17 (Australian currency), $12 (U.S. currency), 7 pounds sterling, $15 (Canadian currency), or Y1300 (Yen). A price reduction of 35% is offered on orders for 8 or more copies. Available from Rumsby Scientific Publishing, P O Box Q355, Q.V.B., Sydney, N.S.W. 2000, Australia. ------- End of forwarded message ------- _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:110>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Wed Oct 20 23:42:54 1993 Date: Thu, 21 Oct 1993 00:49:02 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Software for comparative linguistics (fwd from LINGUIST) To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro New software for comparative linguistics available, forwarded from LINGUIST. Perhaps of interest to some members of Darwin-L. Bob O'Hara darwin@iris.uncg.edu -------------------------------------------------------------------------- LINGUIST List: Vol-4-863. Tue 19 Oct 1993. ISSN: 1068-4875. Date: Tue, 19 Oct 1993 13:04:59 +1000 (EST) From: j.guy@trl.oz.au (Jacques Guy) Subject: Software for PCs I have uploaded today 19 October at garbo.uwasa.fi into their directory pc/incoming a software package for classifying and reconstructing language families from lexicostatistical data and for simulating lexical evolution and borrowing. (It will be moved to their linguistics subdirectory in time). For those of you who have tried lexicostatistics before and found it wanting, I must emphasize now that the tree reconstruction method implemented in this package has nothing in common with glottochronology or lexicostatistics as you remember it. On the very contrary, it expects vocabulary retention to vary wildly from language to language and from time to time. It consists of 5 program files, 3 sample data files, and a documentation file, zipped into GLOTTO01.ZIP (76780 bytes), being: Name Size Contents GLOTMRG.EXE 11088 Program. Merges separate sample wordlists into one, in a format that makes cognate identification easier. GLOTPC.EXE 9392 Program. Input is a file containing identified cognate groups; output is a file containing a table of percentages of shared cognates. GLOTTREE.EXE 23200 Program. Input is a file containing a table of percentages of shared cognates; output is files containing reconstructed tree and table of theoretical cognate percentages. The proportion of vocabulary retained since the preious split is shown on every branch of the reconstructed tree. The table of theoretical percentages gives a means of estimating the reliability of the reconstruction. GLOTED.EXE 24976 An editor for browsing, modifying, and formatting tables of percentages of shared cognates for printing. GLOTSIM.EXE 29904 Program. Input is a file containing the description of the evolution and diversification of a language family or families; output is files containing the log of splits, innovations, borrowings, and percentages of shared cognates. GLOTTO.DOC 51800 Instructions for use. VANUATU.PC 239 Percentages of cognates shared by eight languages of Vanuatu, formerly New Hebrides. VANUATU.SIM 425 Description of the evolution and diversification of a language family. Running GLOTSIM with VANUATU.SIM as input generates a language family with lexicostatistical properties mimicking those of the real languages in VANUATU.PC. UTOAZTEC.PC 2487 Percentages of cognates shared by 32 Uto-Aztecan languages (from W.R. Miller 1984) This package is freeware. There are three main uses to which you can put it. 1. Classifying languages from sample wordlists. 2. Classifying languages from existing tables of cognate percentages. 3. Testing the validity and accuracy of any classification method relying on proportions of shared cognates, including the method implemented in program GLOTTREE. Jacques Guy, Telecom Research Laboratories, 770 Blackburn Road, Clayton 3168, Australia e-mail: j.guy@trl.oz.au -------------------------------------------------------------------------- _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:111>From @gps1.leeds.ac.uk:phl6sf@leeds.ac.uk Thu Oct 21 02:37:45 1993 From: S French <phl6sf@leeds.ac.uk> Subject: Re: mss polymorphism/dog breeds To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu (Patricia Princehouse) Date: Thu, 21 Oct 93 8:38:36 BST Are the textbooks in fact referring to collies, (which are still very much around of course)? Steven French phl6sf@Leeds.ac.uk _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:112>From princeh@husc.harvard.edu Thu Oct 21 17:42:13 1993 Date: Thu, 21 Oct 1993 18:21:50 -0400 (EDT) From: Patricia Princehouse <princeh@husc.harvard.edu> Subject: Re: mss polymorphism/dog breeds To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu On Thu, 21 Oct 1993, S French wrote: > Are the textbooks in fact referring to collies, (which are still > very much around of course)? > > Steven French > phl6sf@Leeds.ac.uk You are undoubtedly right. In my eagerness to find out about an unknown (to me) rare British coal mining terrier (not an unthinkable item as surely there were rats in the mines, weren't there?), I overlooked the obvious possibility that collier was simply a typo for collies (I'm accustomed to seeing the name as a proper noun, Collie or Colley). I now remember that Gould ends the article with a word about a new reconstruction which puts "Eohippus" at @ 50 lbs (roughly Collie-sized) and he makes some inane remark like "Lassie come home." I believe the Collie example is his own, not in any textbook. I guess I serve as an unwitting cultural analogue of a transcription error causing a frame shift. -Patricia Princehouse _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:113>From KESSEL@ACC.FAU.EDU Thu Oct 21 20:44:10 1993 Date: Thu, 21 Oct 1993 21:47 EDT From: Morty Kessel <KESSEL@ACC.FAU.EDU> Subject: Re: mss polymorphism/dog breeds To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu On Mon, 18 Oct 1993 LARRYS@psc.plymouth.edu wrote: > that in General Biology textbooks, when the evolution of the horse is > described, the textbook authors state that Eohippus, one of the ancestral > forms, was the size of a (an I may have this somewhat wrong) collier/ > terrier, a dog from the coal mines of Wales. What is interesting, is the > fact that this dog is no longer a very common breed of dog, yet the > textbook writers rather than mutating the dog into a modern day form of > dog, eg, golden retriever, poodle, etc., continue to use the old name > as referenced in earlier textbooks. This may stray a bit too far from the subject, but as a dog fancier, I would be very interested in any reference you could provide as to this collier/terrier breed used in coal mines. The only reference I know on the textbook subject is Gould's Nat Hist column from @ 1987 which shows the change going from fox (as in Vulpes) to Fox Terrier (a breed still very much around today) and continuing as Fox Terrier in generations of text books. I'm familiar with many rare terriers but have never heard of one from Welsh coal mines. If you can think of any possible reference at all, I'd be eager to hear of it. (by the way, there's nothing new about the Golden Retriever and Poodle. Like the Fox Terrier, these breeds have been around much longer than the taxon Eohippus or even Hyracotherium). Thanks, -Patricia Princehouse Try this reference: Gould, Stephen Jay. 1991. Bully For Brontosaurus. Pp155-167. Morton H. Kessel InterNet: Kessel@acc.fau.edu Department of Anthropology BitNet: Kessel@FAUVAX Florida Atlantic University SoBell: 407/367-3230 Boca Raton, FL 33431-0991 Fax: 407/367-2744 _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:114>From CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU Fri Oct 22 16:47:12 1993 Date: Fri, 22 Oct 93 18:09:31 BS3 From: Charbel Nino El-Hani <CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: Kropotkin, Nowak e May To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Tom Waters asked if anyone thinks Nowak and May's work on spatial prisoner's dilemma has something to do with Kropotkin's book about mutual aid in biological communities (1902). As I have already written in Darwin-L, I think their conclusions are, broadly speaking, very similar. Cooperators survive in the 'struggle for existence' simply because they are able to form groups and, then, to establish *territories*; a good term for this behaviour is *population viscosity*, by Hamilton. As soon as I can, I will try to end an article about the importance of studying the history of a polemics to differentiate clearly the sides in the discussion. In the altruism debate, I think the conflict is between the principle of non- contradiction and the dialectics compromise to contradiction. Depending upon the context in which the selective process takes place, to cooperate and not to cooperate can both lead to survival. I believe that, if the scientific method were based upon dialectical logic, we would advance faster in the production of knowledge. It is only a starting point for a discussion. I will not go further. I would like, as Tom Waters, also to hear (or, better, to read in an incredible distance from the hands which wrote it - Have you thought about the implications of the networks to the scientific enterprise?) about the possible relations of Kropotkin's book to the work of Nowak and May. Charbel Nino El-Hani, Institute of Biology/MsC in Education, Federal University of Bahia, Salvador, Bahia, Brazil. Address: Charbel@BRUFBA. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:115>From CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU Fri Oct 22 17:34:04 1993 Date: Fri, 22 Oct 93 19:28:00 BS3 From: Charbel Nino El-Hani <CHARBEL%BRUFBA.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: Generalizations in Biology To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu In September 24, Brook Milligan wrote: >Over the past year or two I have noticed a series of letters to the editor in Nature justifying the pursuit of systematics and taxonomy as being the basis for generalizations about biological history. It seems that the argument is that by studying the collections present in museums and herbaria, by studying new collections, and by organizing the results of these studies into a historical framework describing which events took place in the past, that we will be able to make generalizations about biology.> We would like to resume this thread asking, as did Milligan, what form this generalization would take. We think the problem in understanding its exact nature is, in part, a problem of defining precisely both the *historical framework* and the word *generalization*. At first sight, we could think about cladistics as one of the possible methodologies for the construction of hypothetical phylogenies, and, historical frameworks for generalizations about biological history. In our discussion, we will refer mainly to cladistics, but we expect most of our doubts to apply to other methods of inferring phylogenies. We consider cladograms to be one of our most powerful tools to understand life's history. It is not so hard to say that they can be a basis for generalizations about it. New and more difficulty questions, however, arise from this statement: Is a cladogram always a hypothesis, or could it originate general conclusions about the biological history? Or, in other words, is cladistics only a methodology for the inference of phylogenies, or a commitment to how things realy occurred in the past? Reification is a common problem in modern science. The analysis of variance, for instance, is only a statistical approach to phenomena, a representation of causes in natural systems as being *primary*, *secondary*, etc., what gives rise to interactions of *first order*, *second order*, etc. Most of the scientists use to reify these numerical components as real forces acting among real objects. In nature, phenomena arise from a totality of causes; analysis of variance is only a method to work with this complexity, and the main causes, the main effects, etc., exist only in the method, not in the reality, where all causes act together, influencing one another in such a manner that it is very difficulty, even impossible, to isolate them. This isolation can be done in laboratory conditions, but the properties of the parts in study, when discovered in experimental conditions, are not the same they have in natural systems, simply because the properties the parts have are determined also by the whole they are in. To answer our question about cladograms, we have to define what is meant by generalizations. Our question is: Can a cladogram be the basis for generalizations *about biological history*? We are not discussing, for instance, the existence of generalizations steps in the construction of a cladogram. Sure, cladistic analysis involve them. When we propose that a group is a monophyletic one we have not observed all the specimens belonging to the group, but only a sample of them; from a particular context of observation, we deduce a general conclusion. We have to see, however, that this generalization is based upon a premise, a Principle of Uniformity. Maybe, this premise can be subject to doubt. Now, let's not think, however, about generalizations into the cladogram, but, on the contrary, about cladograms as generalizations (or basis for them) about biological history. Eldredge and Cracraft (1980, *Phylogenetic Patterns and the Evolutionary Process*) wrote: "the questions arise: to what extent are cladograms to be considered actual representations of phylogeny? The concept of the cladogram has been synonymous with 'phylogeny' in some of the prior literature. The view taken here is that cladograms, in themselves, are not phylogenies, but rather hypothesis about the pattern of nested evolutionary novelties.* They differentiate clearly the *representation* from the *reality*. They do not reify cladograms as being actual phylogenies. In this discussion, we have to refer to the epistemological problem of the cognitive relation. Knowledge is produced in a relation where both the subject and the object interferes. The subject, as an active element in cognitive process, produces a knowledge about the object which is an image, a representation of the object as a material entity, but not a copy of it. The scientist is a real existing man; they have political, ideological positions, they have economical interests, they have, sometimes, a creed, or, who knows, some form of metaphysical conjectures, and all of these properties, qualities of a real man, will influence the scientific knowledge which is produced. So, it seems, by the very properties of cognitive relation, by the constraints of knowing the objects by being *in relation* to them, that we cannot know the world how it really is. We have only images of the world, which are true because they reflect the material existence of the objects in the world, but which are, at the same time, not true, because an image can be a deception, we cannot be sure that the real object is like the image. Mirrors - says Borges - are like labyrinths; they deceive. We cannot reify our images of te world. A cladogram is always a hypothesis about patterns in the evolutionary history, and never a way of constructing generalizations, reifications, about this history. But this is not really a problem, because cladograms, *as hypothesis*, are very useful for the understanding of patterns and processes in phylogenetical history. What do you think? I would like to discuss these themes. Gerson wrote, in September 27, *the historical sciences (and the branches of natural history) don't generalize the same way that the physical sciences do*. We know that the methods of physical sciences were of great influence in the history of the methods in all fields of knowledge. But we also know about the methodological crisis in social and historical sciences. We have, in Darwin-L, to discuss this. To what extent the Cartesian scientific method is valid in the historical sciences? What are the possibilities and limitations of Cartesian method? Charbel Nino El-Hani Institute of Biology/MsC in Education Federal University of Bahia, Salvador, Bahia, Brazil Charbel@BRUFBA Diogo Meyer Institute of Biosciences, University of Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Brazil (today in Stanford University) Diogo@lotka.stanford.edu P.S.: this is an old discussion between Diogo and me. Maybe he disagrees of some the views expressed here. He haven't read this modification of a criticism of him about what I have written. Charbel. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:116>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sun Oct 24 00:22:23 1993 Date: Sun, 24 Oct 1993 01:28:30 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: A direct application of systematic methods to stemmatics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro In response to the question "What's the point of comparing systematics and stemmatics?" Jeff Wills mentioned a project I have been working on with Peter Robinson, and I thought it might be helpful to give a short description of that here, since many people will not be familiar with it. Peter is a manuscript specialist at Oxford who has been working on the history of Old Norse texts for some time. He did his doctoral research on a narrative poem called Svipdagsmal which is known from about 45 manuscript copies written from the late Middle Ages to the early 1800s. For his doctoral work Peter examined all or most of the Svipdagsmal manuscripts, and since he is also interested in computing he created electronic versions of these manuscripts and developed a program called _Collate_ to compare them. From his study of the many copies of this text Peter reconstructed a stemma (a genealogical tree) for Svipdagsmal, showing how all of the known copies are related to one another. Once all the data were in hand the actual process of reconstrucing the stemma took Peter about six months. Although _Collate_ had made direct comparison of the texts fairly efficient, reconstructing the stemma was clearly still a very difficult task. Peter decided to post a challenge on the HUMANIST list (this was about two years ago) to see whether anyone else could take his raw data -- a large table of agreements and disagreements among the Svipdagsmal manuscripts -- and reproduce his stemma by some other means. I saw his challenge and recognized the problem as one very much like phylogeny reconstruction, my own specialty, so I requested a copy of his data. Systematists have developed a number of software packages for cladistic analysis in the last ten years that take tables of data and estimate evolutionary trees from them, and I ran Peter's data through one such program -- PAUP, "Phylogenetic Analysis Using Parsimony" by David Swofford. In about five minutes I produced a tree that was a reasonable approximation of the stemma it had taken Peter six months to reconstruct. PAUP had been developed specifically for the reconstruction of evolutionary trees; no thought whatever had been given to manuscript data or the problems of stemmatics when it was written, and Dave Swofford may not have even known about them at the time. Nevertheless, this program was able to take manuscript data and very quickly approximate the result that Peter had obtained with considerably more effort. The use of this software, developed for research in systematics, now holds considerable promise for stemmatic research, in particular for the reconstruction of large and complex manuscript traditions, such as the Canterbury Tales, where the volume of data makes analysis by inspection very difficult. I also think it holds promise for the reconstruction of language phylogeny, though this is yet to be explored in detail. Here, then, is once concrete example of the value of interdiciplinary interaction among the historical sciences. Peter and I published a preliminary report on our collaboration in the Bryn Mawr Classical Review, and a fuller treatment is in press in the Oxford series Research in Humanities Computing. Full citations are: Robinson, P. M. W., & R. J. O'Hara. 1992. Report on the Textual Criticism Challenge 1992. _Bryn Mawr Classical Review_, 3:331-337. Robinson, P. M. W., & R. J. O'Hara. In press. Cladistic analysis of an Old Norse narrative tradition. _Research in Humanities Computing_, 4. Oxford: Clarendon Press. I have an e-version of the BMCR report, and will try to have it put up in the Darwin-L archives shortly for people to retrieve if they like. If anyone is interested in experimenting with the available systematics software I would recommend getting a copy of _MacClade_, another such program that permits interactive analysis of trees and comes with an excellent introductory manual on cladistic analysis. (PAUP itself is probably a bit stiff for beginners.) The citation for MacClade is: Maddison, Wayne P., & David R. Maddison. 1992. _MacClade: Analysis of Phylogeny and Character Evolution_, Version 3. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates. (ISBN 0-87893-490-1) For people who haven't worked with programs like PAUP before, I append here a sample of the output from Peter's data, just to show what it looks like. The root of the tree (the ancestor) is to the left, and each endpoint (St, J, 11, Gu, etc.) represents an individual Svipdagsmal manuscript. The horizontal length of each branch is proportional to the number of changes taking place along it; the lengths of the vertical lines are arbitrary, and branches may be rotated about nodes arbitrarily as well. This tree is by no means correct in all details; it is an estimate, and it could have been made more precise with additional effort on our part. Our interest in presenting it has just been to show that even with no special attention to the coding of the data it was possible very quickly to come close to the result Peter had obtained earlier. /---- St | /--------- J | /---70 /----- 11 | | \----69 /--- Gu | /----73 \68---- 682 | | | /- 289 | | \--72------- 4877 /--84 | 71---- E | | /---77 /------- L | | | | 74----- 47 | | | | 75 S | | /-----81 \---76 223 | | | | /-------- He | | | \80 /-- 3633 | \-83 \-------------79/--- 6 | | 78---- 818b | | /------- 1870 | \-82------ 34 | /--- Ha | 49---- 215 | /-51 /--------------- 818 /-67 | \50------ 934 | | /-54 /----- 1689 | | | \---53 /- 5 | | /---------55 \---52- 329 | | /---56 \------- 636 | | /--------57 \------ O | | /-58 \------- 1872 | | | \-------------- 2797 /--48 | | /------ 1108 | | | | /-60 /---- 1111 | | | /---65 /----61 \--59-- 165 | | | | | /---62 \------ 1869 | | | | | | \---- 4 | | \-66 \--64 /----- 1609 /47 | | \------63-- 1867 | | | \--------- 1491 | | \------ P 44 | /--------- 1109 || | /45-- 773 || \--46- 1492 |\------- 1868 \---- Ra Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:117>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sun Oct 24 14:20:17 1993 Date: Sun, 24 Oct 1993 15:26:29 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: October 24 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro OCTOBER 24 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1808: CARL BERNHARD VON COTTA is born at Zillbach, Saxe-Weimar-Eisnach (now Germany). Following study at his father's forestry academy and at the Freiberg Bergakademie, the mining school made famous by Werner, Cotta will become professor of geognosy and paleontology at the Bergakademie in 1842. He will travel extensively throughout Europe on geological expeditions, and will publish a genetic system of petrography in 1866, some elements of which remain in use today. Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc. ukans.edu, a network discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. E-mail darwin@iris.uncg.edu for more information. _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:118>From wis@liverpool.ac.uk Mon Oct 25 08:50:18 1993 Date: Mon, 25 Oct 93 13:47:59 GMT To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: wis@liverpool.ac.uk (Bill Sellers) Subject: A quick question... Here's a quick question: who first used the phrase 'caveman' for describing ancestral human forms? I've always thought that it was probably some 19th century palaeontologist, but I'd be really interested in any references anyone can come up with. Thanks Bill Sellers Dept. Human Anatomy, University of Liverpool Remember, it's never too late to have a happy childhood! __________wis@liverpool.ac.uk______________ ( )_( ) / \. ./ __________________/ __=.=__ \ m " m _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:119>From rgmc@henson.cc.wwu.edu Mon Oct 25 10:29:44 1993 Date: Mon, 25 Oct 1993 08:32:08 -0700 (PDT) From: Raymond McInnis <rgmc@henson.cc.wwu.edu> Subject: Re: A quick question... To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Bill, I trust that you have checked the Oxford English Dictionary. It will give the earliest recorded use of the term. Ray McInnis Western Washington U _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:120>From buchignani@hg.uleth.ca Mon Oct 25 11:28:48 1993 Date: Mon, 25 Oct 1993 10:14:36 MDT From: Norman Buchignani <buchignani@hg.uleth.ca> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: A quick question... I'll check my paleo/phys. anth notes for this period and reply later today; as these notes are in a free form database, it'll be straightforward to search them for this particular word. re: the suggestion to check the OED, this can give examples of early use, but should never be relied upon to be definitive. I can give a number of examples I know of where it falls far short here. Norman Buchignani University of Lethbridge _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:121>From peter@usenix.org Mon Oct 25 12:15:19 1993 Date: Mon, 25 Oct 93 10:18:51 PDT From: peter@usenix.org (Peter H. Salus) To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: A quick question... Lubbock, Prehist. Times (1865). for caveman... Peter H. Salus _______________________________________________________________________________ <2:122>From csmem1@abello.seci.uchile.cl Wed Oct 27 00:27:55 1993 Date: Wed, 27 Oct 1993 02:31:15 -0400 From: csmem1@abello.seci.uchile.cl To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: DARWIN CHILE Hello there Recently a big international event took place at the University of Chile in Santiago (CHILE). Eminents scientists met to discuss the stay of Darwin in Chile, between 1833-1835. A wide range of subjects were disscused with emphasis on evolution. The importance of Darwin's observations in Chile was analyzed. An exhibition is being held with importants materials provided by many museums in Chile, Argentina and Great Britain. The organizers of the event had the colaboration of professor Richard Darwin Keynes of the University of Cambridge. For further information please contact Dr. David Yudilevish Dr. David Yudilevish Facultad de Medicina University of Chile fax: (56) 2-7774890 e-mail: dyudilev@abello.cesi.uchile.cl csmem1@abello.cesi.uchile.cl _______________________________________________________________________________ Darwin-L Message Log 2: 90-122 -- October 1993 End