rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 5:71 (January 1994)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<5:71>From delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Thu Jan 13 16:32:15 1994 Date: Thu, 13 Jan 1994 14:16:05 -0800 (PST) From: Scott C DeLancey <delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu> Subject: Re: Systematics and linguistics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Sally Thomason suggests (Tue, 11 Jan 1994) that, given what we know about interlinguistic influence (the various phenomena traditionally rather imprecisely referred to as "borrowing"), we wouldn't expect it to confuse the issue of genetic relationships among languages: > rare. That is: slight to moderate linguistic borrowing -- not > just words, but also sounds, syntax, and even some word structure > -- doesn't obliterate the main lines of descent of a language; > and when borrowing becomes so extreme that the main lines of > descent are seriously obscured, there are usually clues in the > structure of the language. In theory, this is true; as Sally (who, after all, wrote the book on the subject) puts it: > As in biology, linguistic evolution is, as far > as I can tell, mostly non-reticulate...as long as you're > dealing with completely separate languages and not dialects of > the same language, and as long as you are looking at languages > as wholes rather than at individual > linguistic features taken separately. In practice, particularly in the Greenbergian context with which this thread started, I don't think it is, i.e. I disagree that: > So I don't think reticulation, to use the biological terminology > Kent Holsinger was using, is too likely to be a stumbling block -- > at least not often -- in the effort to establish relationships > among languages. Empirically, it has freaquently been, and continues to be, a stumbling block. For some celebrated cases, e.g. the relationships or lack of them among Chinese and various Southeast Asian languages (Vietnamese and languages of the Hmong-Mien and Tai-Kadai families), further work has largely succeeded in unravelling the problem (though respectable and otherwise sensible scholars still try and reopen the issue every now and then). Others, e.g. the problem of the parentage of Japanese, or of the relationship between Indo-European and Afro-Asiatic, remain open and controversial. The problem which recurs again and again is that we find two languages with some substantial amount of what appears to be common vocabulary, that, if they are indeed related, are related so distantly that more conclusive evidence of relationship has been obscured over time. When, as (at least arguably) in the case of Japanese, there is evidence of this sort linking the language to two distinct genetic stocks, there is indeed a stumbling block in the effort to establish relationship. I don't think that, at least in the case of animals, there could be any biological parallel to this situation. And precisely this argument is prominent in discussions of Greenberg's work. Much of his evidence can be discarded on various grounds--bad data, erroneously transcribed data, misanalyzed forms, etc. And undoubtedly a considerable proportion of what's left represents chance resemblance. Greenberg's answer is that even so, he has enough data that there will still be enough left to prove his claims. The problem is that it is still not possible to eliminate borrowing as an explanation for at least some of these data--so the possibility of borrowing again represents a stumbling block. Scott DeLancey delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Department of Linguistics University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!