rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 5:179 (January 1994)
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
This is one message from the Archives of Darwin-L (1993–1997), a professional discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences.
Note: Additional publications on evolution and the historical sciences by the Darwin-L list owner are available on SSRN.
<5:179>From sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu Thu Jan 27 10:58:42 1994 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: History of "adaptation" in historical linguistics Date: Thu, 27 Jan 94 12:05:53 -0500 From: Sally Thomason <sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu> I didn't read email yesterday, so I'm sure I won't be the first linguist to have the pleasure of responding to Bob O'Hara's question about whether linguists ever made claims about particular languages being adapted to their speakers and/or homelands, but I can't resist commenting: the answer is emphatically yes, and the literature on the subject is great fun to read. A good source to start with is Otto Jespersen's book LANGUAGE (1921, if I recall the date correctly): Jespersen doesn't believe any of the wild old theories, but he's close enough to them chronologically that he takes them seriously enough to answer them. When I have time (which I usually don't, unfortunately), I go over some of his examples and counterexamples in my introductory historical linguistics class, in the section on causation of sound change: The Germanic consonant shift (which featured, among other changes, stops becoming fricatives -- p t k > f th x) happened because the Germanic peoples got weak and soft and couldn't pronounce the harsh stops any more; the High German Consonant Shift (which happened later, starting in the south of German-speaking territory, where the mountains are, and which featured partly similar changes) happened because people got so out of breath running up mountains that they couldn't get the complete stops right, and could only gasp out fricatives; harsh climates breed harsh consonant systems (like the Caucasus, with all those wild consonants) -- Jespersen responds that Eskimo territory is pretty harsh in the climate, but Eskimo has only quite gentle sounds; etc., etc. Then there was the very strong 19th-century view that a really good language, like Latin or Greek or (especially, maybe) Sanksrit, was a language with lots of inflectional endings; the modern European languages, with their decayed inflectional systems (compared, of course to highly inflected ancestors), showed moral as well as linguistic decay. Those are probably the most famous sorts of claims. The reason there are more theories about causes of sound change probably has to do with the fact that historical linguists have always known a lot more about sound change than about any other kind of linguistic change -- that's the area where most of the data and most of the theory is. (Of course there is even more information, in a way, about lexical change, but not a lot of theories.) Another big category of speculation doesn't pertain to linguists' speculation, but to laymen's: the theories about what the world's oldest language is. There you get popular prejudices of various kinds about language -- e.g. Andreas Kemke's theory that, in the Garden of Eden, God spoke Swedish (Kemke was a Swede), Adam spoke Danish, and the serpent spoke French. Sally Thomason sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu
Your Amazon purchases help support this website. Thank you!