rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 3: 60–107 — November 1993
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
Darwin-L was an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences, active from 1993–1997. Darwin-L was established to promote the reintegration of a range of fields all of which are concerned with reconstructing the past from evidence in the present, and to encourage communication among scholars, scientists, and researchers in these fields. The group had more than 600 members from 35 countries, and produced a consistently high level of discussion over its several years of operation. Darwin-L was not restricted to evolutionary biology nor to the work of Charles Darwin, but instead addressed the entire range of historical sciences from an explicitly comparative perspective, including evolutionary biology, historical linguistics, textual transmission and stemmatics, historical geology, systematics and phylogeny, archeology, paleontology, cosmology, historical geography, historical anthropology, and related “palaetiological” fields.
This log contains public messages posted to the Darwin-L discussion group during November 1993. It has been lightly edited for format: message numbers have been added for ease of reference, message headers have been trimmed, some irregular lines have been reformatted, and error messages and personal messages accidentally posted to the group as a whole have been deleted. No genuine editorial changes have been made to the content of any of the posts. This log is provided for personal reference and research purposes only, and none of the material contained herein should be published or quoted without the permission of the original poster.
The master copy of this log is maintained in the Darwin-L Archives (rjohara.net/darwin) by Dr. Robert J. O’Hara. The Darwin-L Archives also contain additional information about the Darwin-L discussion group, the complete Today in the Historical Sciences calendar for every month of the year, a collection of recommended readings on the historical sciences, and an account of William Whewell’s concept of “palaetiology.”
----------------------------------------------- DARWIN-L MESSAGE LOG 3: 60-107 -- NOVEMBER 1993 ----------------------------------------------- DARWIN-L A Network Discussion Group on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu is an international network discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. Darwin-L was established in September 1993 to promote the reintegration of a range of fields all of which are concerned with reconstructing the past from evidence in the present, and to encourage communication among academic professionals in these fields. Darwin-L is not restricted to evolutionary biology nor to the work of Charles Darwin but instead addresses the entire range of historical sciences from an interdisciplinary perspective, including evolutionary biology, historical linguistics, textual transmission and stemmatics, historical geology, systematics and phylogeny, archeology, paleontology, cosmology, historical anthropology, historical geography, and related "palaetiological" fields. This log contains public messages posted to Darwin-L during November 1993. It has been lightly edited for format: message numbers have been added for ease of reference, message headers have been trimmed, some irregular lines have been reformatted, and some administrative messages and personal messages posted to the group as a whole have been deleted. No genuine editorial changes have been made to the content of any of the posts. This log is provided for personal reference and research purposes only, and none of the material contained herein should be published or quoted without the permission of the original poster. The master copy of this log is maintained in the archives of Darwin-L by listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. For instructions on how to retrieve copies of this and other log files, and for additional information about Darwin-L, send the e-mail message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. Darwin-L is administered by Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu), Center for Critical Inquiry in the Liberal Arts and Department of Biology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A., and it is supported by the Center for Critical Inquiry, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, and the Department of History and the Academic Computing Center, University of Kansas. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:60>From mayerg@cs.uwp.edu Mon Nov 15 12:21:43 1993 Date: Mon, 15 Nov 1993 11:54:36 -0600 (CST) From: Gregory Mayer <mayerg@cs.uwp.edu> Subject: Re: Haldane and beetles To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu The story of Haldane's conclusions regarding the nature of the Creator brings to mind another famous and possibly apocryphal story about Haldane. Like the story of the beetles, I heard this first as an undergraduate or graduate student; unlike the story of the beetles, I do not know of a published version of the story. It may thus be a purely oral tradition. The story involves Haldane's discovery and quantitative understanding of the theory of kin selection long before W.D. Hamilton's formal development of the theory in 1964. The story goes as follows: Haldane is reputed to have been in a pub one day when conversation turned to the subject of for what someone might be willing to risk their life. The question arose whether someone should risk their life to save a drowning man. The question was put to Haldane, who, after a few moments consideration, including some scribbling on the back of a napkin, replied "No, but I would do it for two brothers or eight cousins." Two sibs or eight first cousins _is_ the break-even point in Hamilton's theory when an act involves loss of life to the aid donor but the saving of the lives of the aid recipients. James Crow (_Basic Concepts in Population, Quantitative, and Evolutionary Genetics_, Freeman, 1986) notes that Haldane was aware of the principles of kin selection, but that it was not developed until Hamilton. The story might be an explanation, perhaps even correct, for how Haldane came to appreciate kin selection. If there is no published source for this story, then it is an oral tradition passed on to students of evolution over several decades (Haldane died in 1964), and across at least two continents. Gregory C. Mayer mayerg@cs.uwp.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:61>From azlerner@midway.uchicago.edu Mon Nov 15 12:26:31 1993 Date: Mon, 15 Nov 93 12:30:01 CST From: "asia z lerner" <azlerner@midway.uchicago.edu> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: grad school Folks, I have a friend who is interested in pursuing grad studies in philosophy of biology (with major interests in ethology). Can any of you recommend any programs which might cater to these interests? There's the University of Chicago, with Robert Richards and William Wimsatt. Asia _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:62>From ronald@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu Mon Nov 15 15:03:52 1993 Date: Mon, 15 Nov 93 11:07:16 HST From: Ron Amundson <ronald@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Haldane's cousins The 2 brothers or 8 cousins story is told in S.J. Gould's "So Cleverly Kind an Animal," which is in _Ever Since Darwin_. Ron Amundson ronald@uhunix.uhcc.hawaii.edu ronald@uhunix.bitnet _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:63>From r_king@academic.cc.colorado.edu Mon Nov 15 19:02:00 1993 Date: Mon, 15 Nov 1993 18:03:57 MST From: The Dread Pirate Robert <r_king@cc.colorado.edu> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: RE: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. class. I'm an undergrad student majoring in history. Mostly out of curiousity, I'd like professional definitions for cladistics, evolutionary classification, etc., as well. Thanks. Robert King _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:64>From ahouse@hydra.rose.brandeis.edu Mon Nov 15 19:37:16 1993 Date: Mon, 15 Nov 1993 20:42:40 -0500 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: ahouse@hydra.rose.brandeis.edu (Jeremy John Ahouse) Subject: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. ref >I'm an undergrad student majoring in history. Mostly out of curiousity, I'd >like professional definitions for cladistics, evolutionary classification, >etc., as well. Thanks. > > Robert King I think the folks who asked will find Elliot Sober's _Reconstructing the Past_ helpful (he doesn't in the end come down on the side of cladistics, still his descriptions seem fair). You might also want to check out Mark Ridley's _Evolution and Classification_. - jeremy ::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::: Jeremy John Ahouse Biology Dept. & Center for Complex Systems Brandeis University Waltham, MA 02254-9110 (617) 736-4954 email: ahouse@hydra.rose.brandeis.edu Mail from Mac by Eudora 1.3.1 RIPEM/PGP accepted. "Si un hombre nunca se contradice, sera porque nunca dice nada" - Miguel de Unamuno _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:65>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Mon Nov 15 19:57:56 1993 Date: Mon, 15 Nov 1993 21:04:58 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Re: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. class. To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro Lynn Hanninen asks some large but important questions. I don't know whether I'll succeed in answering them briefly, but I'll give it a try. Since they do require a bit of explanation I'll answer in installments over a day or two, rather than all at once. The first question is about the distinction between phenetics, cladistics, and evolutionary classification. The most important thing one has to do before dealing with this question is to clearly distinguish between (1) classification and (2) phylogenetic inference (reconstructing the evolutionary tree). Failure to distinguish between these two different activities has been a source of much confusion. Phenetics and cladistics can be approaches to _either_ classification or phylogeny reconstruction; "evolutionary classification" is (as the name suggests) an approach to classification, not phylogeny reconstruction. First let's talk about phylogenetic inference. Suppose we have three species, A, B, and C. They may be related in any one of the following ways. (It is important to understand that "relationship" in this context means historical, genealogical relationship: relative recency of common ancestry. It is also important to understand that these trees are "trees of history"; that is, the root represents an ancestor that actually existed at sometime in the past.) /------- C /------- C /------- B /-----| /-----| /-----| / \------- B / \------- A / \------- A ----| ----| ----| \ \ \ \------------- A \------------- B \------------- C Now since history only happened one way, the question is: Which of these trees is the correct representation of the history of the species in question? A phenetic approach to the problem of phylogenetic inference would say that the pair of species that are _most similar_ are the most closely related. Thus if B and C are more similar to one another than either is to A, then B and C are more closely related to one another than either is to A, and so the tree on the left represents the true phylogeny. Advocates of this approach have proposed various ways of calculating "overall similarity". The cladistic approach to the problem is very different. It decomposes the phenetic notion of "overall similarity" into two parts: (1) _ancestral_ or primitive similarity, and (2) _derived_ similarity. The cladistic approach to phylogenetic inference would say that the two species out of these three that share the greatest number of _derived_ similarities are the most closely related (ancestral similarities being irrelevant). Thus if A and B have more derived similarties in common with one another that either does with C, then A and B are more closely related to one another than either is to C. The correct tree in that case would the one on the right. Now what is the difference between ancestral and derived similarity? That is a topic I'll save for another installment, but I'll just say simply here that a derived similarity (or derived character state) is an _innovation_, and a ancestral similarity (or ancestral character state) is a _retention_. ("Ancestral character" and "derived character" are somewhat lax synonyms of "ancestral character state" and "derived character state".) More on the ancestral/derived distinction later. Let's now switch from phylogenetic inference to classification. I should state my own position clearly at this point: I regard the distinction between phylogenetic inference and classification to be vitally important because it allows systematists to ignore the whole subject of classification, on which an enormous amount of ink has been wasted. Consequently, I regard the discussion from this point on as moot: its value lies only in helping us to understand some of the confused literature of the past. I am fully aware that there are people who don't share this view, but I don't propose to debate them on this point. Suppose we still have the same three species, A, B, and C, and we have determined that this is the correct phylogeny: /------- B /-----| / \------- A ----| \ \------------- C The question facing someone interested in _classifying_ these three species might run something like this. (Notice that there is no dispute about the phylogeny; that has already been established.) Which of these is the best classification for the three species? genus genus genus species A species A species B species B species C species C genus genus genus species C species B species A (There are actually a great variety of other possibilities, especially if we allow the rank to vary, including a genus for each species, one genus for all three, separate families, etc., but this is a simplified example.) Remember that all parties agree on what the phylogeny is, namely that A and B are coordinate branches (sister clades). A phenetic approach to _classification_ would say that, since B and C are more similar to one another than either is to A, then B and C should be grouped together into the same genus, so the right-hand classification is best. A cladistic approach to _classification_ would say that all groups in the classification must be whole branches (clades) in the tree, so the left-hand classification is the best one, since it puts A and B in the same genus. The approach called "evolutionary classification", which has had Ernst Mayr as its most vocal proponent, is something of a combination between the two. It would say that the groups in the classification should be clades unless there have been highly unequal rates of evolution in different branches, in which case the branch that has undergone a lot of change should be singled out and given higher rank (a family or order perhaps, rather than a genus). Evolutionary classifications and phenetic classifications are often similar as a result of this. Consider the following as an example of a true phylogeny (which it is) for three species: /------- lungfish /-----| / \------- cow ----| \ \------------- goldfish An advocate of phenetic classification might say that a goldfish and a lungfish are more similar to one another than either is to the cow, and so should be grouped together in the class Pisces. An advocate of evolutionary classification might come to more or less the same conclusion, on the grounds that the cow's lineage has been highly modified since it diverged from the lungfish's lineage, and so the cow has "left behind" the two fish (as Pisces) and entered its own class, Mammalia. An advocate of cladistic classification, however, would say that while it is true that overall the goldfish and the lungfish are more similar, the lungfish and the cow are in fact a clade, and only clades should be recognized in a classification, in this case perhaps as the group Choanata. Cladistic _classification_ thus rejects the traditional taxon "Pisces", and this example (among others such as the rejection of the traditional taxon "Reptilia") caused many systematists to recoil from it. The reason the situation arises in the first place is that _classifications_ existed long before evolution was accepted, and pre-evolutionary classifications were by-and-large based on something like overall similarity; that is, they were loosely phenetic. Once the theory of descent was accepted it became apparent that some of the pre-evolutionary _groups_ are in fact not _clades_ (whole branches of the tree). If you read the discussion of systematics in the _Origin of Species_ you can see this conflict becoming apparent to Darwin as he attempts to reconcile established ideas of classification with his new idea of "genealogical arrangement" (phylogeny). But keep in mind that if phylogeny -- evolutionary history -- is what one is interested in then one can disregard much of what has been said on the subject of classification as a distinct enterprise. Some general introductions to the issues discussed above are listed in the file "biblio.clades" in the Darwin-L archives. If you send the message GET DARWIN-L BIBLIO.CLADES to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu a copy will be sent to you. More on Lynn's other questions in a later post. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:66>From SHANKSN@ETSUSERV.EAST-TENN-ST.EDU Tue Nov 16 08:13:26 1993 From: <SHANKSN@ETSUSERV.EAST-TENN-ST.EDU> Organization: East Tennessee State University To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Date: Tue, 16 Nov 1993 09:11:44 GMT-5 Subject: Re: grad school > Date sent: Mon, 15 Nov 1993 15:01:43 -0600 > From: "asia z lerner" <azlerner@midway.uchicago.edu> > To: Multiple recipients of list <darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu> > Subject: Re: grad school > > Folks, I have a friend who is interested in pursuing grad studies in > philosophy of biology (with major interests in ethology). Can any of > you recommend any programs which might cater to these interests? > > There's the University of Chicago, with Robert Richards and William Wimsatt. > Asia Many thanks, I'll pass the advice along....Niall Shanks _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:67>From @KENTVM.KENT.EDU:KHERMANN@KENTVM.KENT.EDU Tue Nov 16 09:51:10 1993 Date: Tue, 16 Nov 1993 10:46:49 -0500 (EST) From: "Kenneth W. Hermann" <KHERMANN%KENTVM.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: Information on George Maw To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Does anyone on the list have any background information on George Maw, a rather obscure editor of the *Zoologist* in the 1860s. I am curious to know more about him because of the high regard which Darwin had for his review of *The Origin of Species* for the Zoologist and the several letters they exchanged concerning the issues Maw raised in that review. I have not been able to locate any additional information in either the *DNB* or *DSB*. I have our ILL staff diligently tracking down an obscure pamphlet of his. Any help would be greatly appreciated. Kenn Hermann History Dept Kent State Univ. khermann@kentvm.kent.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:68>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Tue Nov 16 10:28:09 1993 Date: Tue, 16 Nov 1993 11:35:03 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Re: Information on George Maw To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro Regarding George Maw: The following comes from _Charles Darwin: A Companion_, by R.B. Freeman (Wm. Dawson & Sons Ltd., 1978). It's not much, but perhaps a start. This is a paraphrase: Maw, George. 1832-1912. Geologist and botanist. Of Benthall Green. July 1861 Maw reviewed the Origin in _Zoologist_. Darwin to Lyell: "evidently a thoughtful man" (Life & Letters, 2:376). Maw provided _Drosophyllum_ for Darwin's _Insectivorous Plants_. Bob O'Hara darwin@iris.uncg.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:69>From jlipps@ucmp1.Berkeley.EDU Tue Nov 16 10:54:30 1993 Date: Tue, 16 Nov 93 08:59:35 PST From: jlipps@ucmp1.Berkeley.EDU (Jere Lipps) To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. class. David Lindberg has a real simple explanation in "Fossil Prokaryotes and Protists", chapter 4 (Blackwell, 1992; edited by me). Jere H. Lipps _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:70>From TQAF072%UTXVM.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU Tue Nov 16 11:33:20 1993 Date: Tue, 16 Nov 1993 11:35 -0500 (CDT) From: SShelton@UTXVM.CC.UTEXAS.EDU Subject: taxidermy To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu I realize that this may seem to come from far, far left field. BUT...I am chairing a session at the March Texas Association of Museums meeting in Houston on the taxidermy/trophy mount dilemma (you can't use them, you can't easily store them and you can't give them away), and I am also working on a manuscript on grotesques. What I am looking for is earliest refences to, and history of, taxidermy and suchlike preservation methods for natural history specimens. I am interested in very specific techniques as well as in the underlying philosophy and psychology (the latter is especially interesting with regard to grotesques). Any/all help appreciated; resulting bibliography will be posted. Many thanks. Sally Shelton Texas Memorial Museum tqaf072@utxvm.cc.utexas.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:71>From maisel@Sdsc.Edu Tue Nov 16 12:51:17 1993 Date: Tue, 16 Nov 93 18:54:33 GMT From: maisel@Sdsc.Edu (Merry Maisel, 619-534-5127) Subject: Beetled Browse To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu It's probably a nasty habit, like collecting matchbooks, but I save my copies of _Natural History_ until Gould's collections of essays appear. You should know that he does not just thrust a year's worth of columns into a book, which he could well do, given the way his stuff sells. He makes very deliberate choices and rearrangements. I don't know if he will put his January column, "A Special Fondness for Beetles," in the next collection, but I hope he does. Then we shall all have, in more permanent form, a very nice consideration of the problem of the little academic teehee passed from generation to generation. As Gould points out in the column, the latest resurrection of the line from Haldane was in a review of a meeting by one who attended, Robert May of Oxford, published in _Nature_ in late 1989: May began his article: "Haldane's best-remembered remark, that God has `an inordinate fondness for beetles," was elicited by Jowett's question, at high table at Balliol, as to what his studies had revealed about the deity." This elicited a flurry of letters to _Nature_, one of which pointed out that Jowett had died when Haldane was a year old and hence the conversation at high table could not have taken place. To this, May properly replied, "Mundane constraints of time and space do not apply to stories about Oxford." Gould makes the point that the best one-liners are often attributed to those, already famous, who make a good story better. (If you've ever had one of your best lines appropriated in this way, you will know the frustration of being unable to secure a proper attribution to yourself.) And he discusses beetles and how they are counted. Great column. Merry Maisel science writer, San Diego Supercomputer Center grad student, Science Studies, UC San Diego maisel@sdsc.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:72>From acvascon@ibase.br Tue Nov 16 14:05:59 1993 From: acvascon@ibase.br Date: Tue, 16 Nov 93 18:07:32 BDB To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Weights A very common feature in recent cladistics softwares is the possibility of weighting characters according to the level of homoplasy (Henning86, Paup 3.0, Pee-Wee). For classificatory purposes it's sometimes a very good strategy, but what does it mean in biological sense? How can we perceive the difference of a character with a low or a higth weight in the real world? _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:73>From mayerg@cs.uwp.edu Tue Nov 16 14:39:55 1993 Date: Tue, 16 Nov 1993 13:49:34 -0600 (CST) From: Gregory Mayer <mayerg@cs.uwp.edu> Subject: Re: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. class. To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Bob O'Hara's first installment of his reply to Lynn Hanninen's query is a clear exposition of many of the issues involved, and one with with which I find myself in almost total agreement, especially on the importance of distinguishing phylogenetic inference (=historical reconstruction) from classification (=a set of names and their definitions). It is with some trepidation, therefore, that I venture to suggest an amendment to his characterization of phenetics as a method of phylogenetic inference. (In fact, it might be better for those encountering these issues for the first time to read O'Hara's full exposition first before returning to my suggestion here.) My suggestion is that phenetics is best characterized as _not_ being a method of phylogenetic inference at all. It is quite right to say that phenetics is concerned with overall similarity. But it is not concerned with phylogenetic inference. Let me illustrate this with two lines from Sneath & Sokal's _Numerical Taxonomy_ (Freeman, San Francisco, 1973; the classic text of phenetics): Taxonomic relationships are evaluated purely on the basis of resemblances existing _now_ in the material at hand. [p. 9, emphasis in original] The separation of overall similarity (phenetics) from evolutionary branching sequences (cladistics) is an important advance in taxonomic thinking. [p. 10] The distinction between phenetics and phylogenetics is clearly acknowledged, and phylogenetic intent is disavowed. Part of my trepidation in raising this issue is that phenetics has a complex history, and its aims have shifted over time; I do not wish to rehash these history-of-the-discipline matters. And if this were my only quibble with O'Hara, i.e. as to what it was the pheneticists actually wanted to do, I would not bother to mention it. What I think _is_ worth mentioning is that phylogenetics is an intention, not a technique. What distinguishes a phylogenetic method from a phenetic method is that a phylogenetic method produces an estimate of history. As O'Hara notes, history happened only one way, and there is thus a "parametric" history to be inferred. Some of our estimates of this history will be good, and some will be bad, and one reason that an estimate could be bad is that the method of inference is not very good. I think this is what O'Hara meant when he contrasted overall similarity with an analysis based on the distinction between derived and ancestral character states: he meant that, in general, overall similarity is not as good a method of inference. Methods of historical inference are justified on the basis of their ability to reconstruct history. Depending on the historical process, and the data available, different methods may be best. Given a particular historical process, someone might argue that, say, single-linkage clustering of Jaccard coefficients of similarity gives the best estimate of the history. That single-linkage clustering and Jaccard coefficients are used by pheneticists does not make the method phenetic; it is phylogenetic because it is a method of inferring history. Now it might be a bad method, but that depends on the nature of the historical process. A statistical analogy may make the point clearer. One way to estimate the mean of a distribution is to rank all the observations and choose the value of the central observation as the estimate of the mean. This is a method of inference. If we have good reason for believing that the distribution is symmetrical, then this is not a bad method (not the best, but not bad). If the distribution is not symmetrical, then this is a bad method. The method is better or worse depending on the nature of the distribution. Let me conclude with an example where many biologists have argued that overall similarity _is_ a good estimate of phylogeny. Some molecular evolutionists have concluded that, at the molecular level, evolution proceeds at a constant rate at a given locus. If this is true, and if evolution is divergent, then the overall similarity at a locus _is_ proportional to the time since common ancestry, and an average-linkage tree of the overall similarities will be a good estimate of the phylogeny. The premises of the justification may be false, in which case the technique will be bad as a method of historical inference. Studies have been done of exactly how untrue the premises must be before the method fails. But if the premises are true, then it will be a good method. Thus I endorse the clear distinction of phylogenetic inference and classification. But phenetics and evolutionary classification are both about classification. Within phylogenetics, many techniques are or have been advocated; what makes them phylogenetic is that their proponents intend them to be estimates of phylogenetic history. We may judge a technique as more or less successful, but they are phylogenetic nonetheless. Gregory C. Mayer mayerg@cs.uwp.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:74>From KIMLER@social.chass.ncsu.edu Thu Nov 18 11:09:18 1993 From: KIMLER@social.chass.ncsu.edu To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Date: Wed, 17 Nov 1993 14:54:49 EST5EDT Subject: Momentum There has been much discussion of the validity of biological selectionist analogies and models for the historical treatment of cultural change. I am interested in metaphors derived from physics. Early 19th-century Naturphilosophie and German theories of history seem to draw on physical notions of polarity and the resolution of tensions (explicitly found in Kant). Herbert Spencer used his own version of the new theory of conservation of energy, calling it "persistence of force". From this he inferred a world of matter and motion in a constant equilibration, producing a process in the direction of increasing "heterogeneity" out of unstable "homogeneity". What about momentum? The early evolutionary paleontologists constructed a theory of phylogenetic inertia to explain trends. Can someone provide examples and/or references to the use of the notions of momentum or inertia in constructing a model of cultural change? William Kimler Dept. History North Carolina State University KIMLER@NCSU.EDU _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:75>From eshaw@huh.harvard.edu Thu Nov 18 11:42:08 1993 From: "Elizabeth Shaw" <eshaw@huh.harvard.edu> Date: Wed, 17 Nov 93 16:34:15 EDT To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: more on maw George Maw (1832-1912) was an interesting fellow. He owned a pottery in Shropshire - perhaps Darwin knew him through the family interests in the business. When he had accumulated enough money Maw seemed to allow his interests turn to natural history - in particular, to botany. He travelled quite a bit in Europe & in N. Africa and eventually in 1886 published a monograph of the genus Crocus. There are several obits of Maw; e. g., in Kew Bulletin 1912:155. cheers betsy shaw Elizabeth A. Shaw Harvard University Herbaria 22 Divinity Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138 USA 617-495-1939 FAX: 617-661-3751 eshaw@huh.harvard.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:76>From rxn106@cac.psu.edu Thu Nov 18 14:12:26 1993 From: RICARDO NASSIF <rxn106@cac.psu.edu> Subject: Darwin biographies To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Date: Thu, 18 Nov 1993 15:15:27 -0500 (EST) Would someone in this list kindly post a brief list of C. Darwin biographies? Ranking them would be great too. Thanks. .rn ricardo nassif rxn106@cac.psu.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:77>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Fri Nov 19 22:15:46 1993 Date: Fri, 19 Nov 1993 23:23:43 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Momentum and other physical metaphors in history To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro I thought William Kimler's question about the notion of "momentum" in history and the historical sciences was a fascinating one, and I regret that I don't have anything to contribute in answer to it. I hope others will, and maybe William would consider posting some notes on early ideas of phylogenetic "inertia." The only vaguely related notion I could think of (another application of a semi-physical concept to natural history) was the notion of historical "polarity" advocated by the early Victorian naturalist Edward Forbes (1815- 1854). What little I know of this idea comes from Janet Browne's book _The Secular Ark: Studies in the History of Biogeography_ (Yale University Press, 1983). According to Browne, Forbes believed that taxa replaced one another through time, such that when one was on the wane another was rising to take its place: "Because this sequence of events described a maximum-minimum-maximum story, Forbes argued that the development of life proceeded in two divergent directions: 'The relation between the Palaeozoic and Neozoic life-assemblages is one of development in opposite directions, in other words, of Polarity.'" (Browne, p. 153) Browne reproduces some circular and hour-glass shaped diagrams Forbes used to illustrate this notion of polarity. I hope some other people may be able to follow this thread on the application of physical ideas/terms/metaphors to the historical sciences. Are there any example in linguistics of concepts like force, polarity, or momentum? Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:78>From ronald@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu Sat Nov 20 02:43:33 1993 Date: Fri, 19 Nov 93 22:47:01 HST From: Ron Amundson <ronald@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: Momentum and other physical metaphors in history On Forbes and polarity, there's Phillip Rehbock's _The Philosophical Naturalists_. Also discusses Owen's use of 'polarity,' as I recall. Those uses of polarity did not strike me as closely related to physical (e.g. magnetic) polarity -- at least at the shallow depth at which I understood them. Ron Amundson ronald@uhunix.uhcc.hawaii.edu ronald@uhunix.bitnet _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:79>From CSM@macc.wisc.edu Sat Nov 20 14:24:28 1993 Date: Sat, 20 Nov 93 14:26 CDT From: Craig McConnell <CSM@macc.wisc.edu> Subject: Momentum To: DARWIN-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu This is in response to Will Kimler's query of 18 November, "Can someone provide examples and or references to the use of notions of momentum or inertia in constructing a model of cultural change?" I have a tangential response, which may impinge on his question. This is more a response to the question, "Where are the metaphors from physics in biology?" I have noticed in my survey of the history of modern biology that references to idealized Newtonian science get fewer and farther between the closer one gets to the present. Lamarck and Cuvier, for example, sound very Newtonian in their emphasis on axiomatic science. Schwann made nice comparisons between the regular laws of the solar system and the regular laws of cellular development. As you get closer to (and pass by) Darwin, the language of mechanism is still there, but the metaphors to force, law, etc. (a group that would include momentum) fall out. So Haeckel has his idea of a universal theory of development that can be discerned in both the organic and inorganic world, but he's imposing biology lingo on physics more often than not. Weismann's criteria for a good theory of heredity includes a mechanistic flavor, but it too is cast in biology lingo. My tentative hypothesis at this stage is that the unfolding complexity of biology begins to preclude simplistic comparisons to physics (I once heard a brilliant lecturer ask, rhetorically I'm certain, "How could physicists _not_ figure out the orbit of a planet? It's two objects in empty space! Now explain where life comes from: _that's_ a problem!"). I would say a corollary to this is that as biologists became more sophisticated in their thinking, they found that organic metaphors were more convincing than physical ones. (I just read Frederick Churchill, "From Heredity Theory to Vererbung" _Isis_, 1987--he argues that as biologists like Weismann and Hertwig got more savvy about heredity, they relied less frequently on economic metaphors). So my question for you is: isn't it more likely that you'll find organic metaphors for cultural change than momentum metaphors? Craig S. McConnell, (608) 238-1352 Internet: csm@macc.wisc.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:80>From leh1@Lehigh.EDU Sat Nov 20 15:41:31 1993 Date: Sat, 20 Nov 1993 16:17:36 EST From: leh1@Lehigh.EDU (Lynn E. Hanninen) Subject: Re: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. class. To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Hi! I have 2 questions regarding cladistics. Both questions arise from reading Mayr. 1) My understanding was that Mayr said there is no method in Cladistics for going from phylogenetic reconstruction to classification. Comments? 2) My understanding is that a major purpose of cladistics is to determine -branch points.- What EXACTLY is a branch point? Is cladistics concerned with determining branch points? lynn (still trying) ************************** Lehigh office: rm. 221, CU #17 office phone #: (215) 758-3662 home phone #: (215) 758-1367 e-mail: leh1@lehigh.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:81>From WILLS@macc.wisc.edu Sat Nov 20 18:46:39 1993 Date: Sat, 20 Nov 93 18:48 CDT From: Jeffrey Wills <WILLS@macc.wisc.edu> Subject: Physical metaphors in linguistics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu I think the primary use of physical metaphors in linguistics comes from an assumption that language (an animated abstract) tries to preserve a uniform contour or density. If you check back to early messages on this list you will find, I think, descriptions of language changing to correct an "imbalance" or to fill a "gap". The principle "natura abhorret vacuum" may not arise from physics (I know it as a line from Spinoza and I'm sure there are biological parallels), but it does easily lead to other physical metaphors. In Linguistics, the analysis of language as a system led to the study of different "functions" within the system (Jakobson, who also uses the language of "poles" in poetics, and members of the Prague School who study phonological features which have positive or negative valences, as it were). In particular, I think of a man in this tradition named Andre' Martinet who taught in Paris and then at Columbia. Instead of the teleological terms familiar to the Prague School he describes the tendency of language change to strive toward economy in reconciling two opposite needs: toward efficiency in communication (as many units as possible, as different from each other as possible) and minimum effort (as few units as possible, as similar as possible). Success is measured in terms of distributing functional yield or functional "load". This doesn't involve very fancy physics but does use the language of mechanics. The best place to go might be Martinet's 1955 _Economies des changements phonetiques_. In it he describes the "pressure" in the "chain" for maximum differentiation and "equidistance". Remember that the physiological basis of the production of sounds inherently involves motor movements which have energy, frequency, height,vibration,inertia andother elements of physics. So within the system (or the map of the physical mouth or other features) we can have "empty holes" which are likely to be filled by new phonemes or whatever. Martinet later went on to apply this to syntax as well. Chains can push or pull. In semantics, there is similarly talk of forming and filling vacuums (or will you allow me vacua?). I note Geoffrey Hughes (_Words in Time_, 1988, p. 12): "Certain areas of the vocabulary perennially generate specialization. As the explicit terms for sexual activity become unacceptable and then taboo, numerous general latinized words were drawn into the 'semantic vacuum'. Among them were *rape* (1482), *consummation* (1530), *seduce* (1560), *erection* (1594), . . . *orgasm* (1684), *intercourse* (1798), *climax* (1918), *ejaculation* (1927)...." The idealized distribution of meanings (into which vacuums come and go) was even measured by the psychologist G. K. Zipf. Perhaps not surpringly "the different meanings of a word will tend to be equal to the square root of its relative frequency" (1945 article in Journal of Gen. Psychology). Jeffrey Wills wills@macc.wisc.edu Univ.of Wisc.-Madison _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:82>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sun Nov 21 13:31:05 1993 Date: Sun, 21 Nov 1993 14:39:02 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: November 21 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro NOVEMBER 21 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1881: AMI BOUE dies at Voslau, Austria. Born in Hamburg, Germany, Boue had declined to enter his family's shipping business and had instead emigrated to Scotland at the age of twenty. He studied geology, botany, and medicine at the University of Edinburgh, and eventually returned to the Continent where he participated in the founding of the Societe Geologique de France in 1830. Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L, an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. For information send the message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:83>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sun Nov 21 20:18:22 1993 Date: Sun, 21 Nov 1993 21:26:13 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Darwin's lost Galapagos fossils To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro This message comes from Jere Lipps who was having difficulty posting it. Please reply either to him or to the group as a whole. Bob O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) ---------- When Darwin sailed into the Galapagos, one of the ship's crewmen climbed Cerro Brujo on San Cristobal and returned with some fossils. Darwin reported this (3d edition of Geological Observations, 1896, p. 130) and used the occurrence of these fossils as evidence that the tuff cone of Cerro Brujo had been uplifted, although he acknowledged that they could have also been carried up to their height (of several hundred feet) by the eruption. In 1986, Carole Hickman and I together with several others collected many fossils from Cerro Brujo. We'd like to write up this occurrence and have searched the Natural History Museum, London, Oxford U., Cambridge U., and the Geol. Soc. of London for the material Darwin wrote about. No luck. No one seems to know where the fossils are. Does anyone out there have any ideas? Jere H. Lipps (jlipps@ucmp1.Berkeley.EDU) Museum of Paleontology UC Berkeley _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:84>From chladil@geo.geol.utas.edu.au Sun Nov 21 21:07:03 1993 From: Mark Chladil <chladil@geo.geol.utas.edu.au> Subject: beetles....thanks To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu (darwin-l) Date: Mon, 22 Nov 93 14:10:29 EST Thank you all who replied to my request re beetles, Haldane etc. In summary see S.J. Gould in Natural History January 1993. G.E. Hutchinson's 1958 Homage to Santa Rosalia is also a good read but is not after all about Haldane, beetles or the Creator.... It was an interesting diversion from carabid taxonomy and communities. Cheers Mark _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:85>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Mon Nov 22 00:05:42 1993 Date: Mon, 22 Nov 1993 01:13:35 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: November 22 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro NOVEMBER 22 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1787: RASMUS KRISTIAN RASK is born at Braendekilde, Denmark. Following two years of study in Iceland, Rask will publish _Undersogelse om det gamle Nordiske eller Islandske Sprogs Oprindelse_ (_Investigation on the Origin of the Old Norse or Icelandic Language_, 1818), which will demonstrate the relationship of the Scandinavian languages to Latin and Greek. He will later bring the Celtic languages into the Indo-European family, and will recognize that Basque and Finno-Ugaric are independent of this group. Rask will master more than 25 languages by the time of his death in 1832, and he will be remembered as one of the founders of comparative Indo-European linguistics. Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L, an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. For information send the message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:86>From rxn106@cac.psu.edu Mon Nov 22 09:05:34 1993 From: RICARDO NASSIF <rxn106@cac.psu.edu> Subject: History of evolutionary thought To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Date: Mon, 22 Nov 1993 10:08:59 -0500 (EST) First, I would like to thank whoever is in charge of the "Today in historical...". These small pieces are very informative (at least for the non-specialist) and I really look forward to receiving it. Second, I posted sometime ago a request for a list of Darwin (and related) biographies but so far got no answer. It doesn't have to include price, ISBN, etc: Just a simple list, if possible ranked in terms of "quality" in someone's opinion. It would also be nice mentioning if the item is in print. Cheers & thanks. .rn _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:87>From mayerg@cs.uwp.edu Mon Nov 22 10:12:26 1993 Date: Mon, 22 Nov 1993 09:42:51 -0600 (CST) From: Gregory Mayer <mayerg@cs.uwp.edu> Subject: Re: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. class. To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu > 1) My understanding was that Mayr said there is no method in Cladistics for > going from phylogenetic reconstruction to classification. Comments? Sort of true. Cladistic classifications reflect only recency of common ancestry (as opposed to degree of divergence). This imposes a strong constraint on such classifications, if they are to be of the traditional Linnaean style. The constraint is that sister groups (i.e. the pair of clades descending from a single common ancestor) must be of the same rank. Thus for a particular phylogeny, many possible classifications will not satisfy this constraint, and thus will not be cladistic. However, many possible classifications _will_ satisfy this constraint; there is thus no procedure that moves one ineluctably from a tree to some single classification, which alone is cladistic. The cladist, while being constrained more than a pheneticist or evolutionary classificationist (sorry for the barbarism, but evolutionist without some reference to classification would not be right) would be, must still make decisions as to which rank sister taxa should be, and which sister taxa should be named (in a practical system, not all sister taxa can be named). This is what Mayr meant by saying cladistics had no method. > 2) My understanding is that a major purpose of cladistics is to determine > -branch points.- What EXACTLY is a branch point? Is cladistics concerned > with determining branch points? Evolutionary events are often divided into two sorts: anagenesis, or changes within a single lineage, and cladogenesis, or the splitting of a single lineage into two or more lineages. Here's an example, which might, in fact, be wrong, but it illustrates the point. Ribbon snakes occurred across the eastern part of the U.S. During one of the climatic vicissitudes of the Pleistocene, acceptable habitat became restricted to the east (toward Florida) and the west (toward Texas), separating the ribbon snakes into two geographic areas. In the separate eastern and western areas, evolution continued , but independently. The snakes changed a bit in their coloration and scales and probably other ways. Thus anagenesis occurred in the isolated populations. When the climate ameliorated, snakes moved back into once-again favorable areas along the Mississippi. When eastern snakes and western snakes met, however, they could no longer freely reproduce with one another. They had become separate species, i.e. independently evolving lineages. Cladogenesis had occurred. The complex of events leading to the splitting of the ribbon snake into two species, the eastern and western ribbon snakes, is what is referred to as the branching point: the point (although, of course, in the postulated example it was not a _point_ in time, but a sequence of events) at which a single branch of the phylogenetic tree becomes two branches. The goal of cladistics (as a branch of phylogenetics, as opposed to classification) is to discover the sequence in which these branching events occurred. Gregory C. Mayer mayerg@cs.uwp.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:88>From LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU Mon Nov 22 10:49:00 1993 Date: Mon, 22 Nov 1993 10:49:00 -0600 From: "JOHN LANGDON" <LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU> To: rxn106@cac.psu.edu, darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: History of evolutionary thought In message <199311221509.AA19147@wilbur.cac.psu.edu> writes: > First, I would like to thank whoever is in charge of the > "Today in historical...". These small pieces are very informative (at > least for the non-specialist) and I really look forward to receiving > it. > > Second, I posted sometime ago a request for a list of Darwin > (and related) biographies but so far got no answer. It doesn't have to > include price, ISBN, etc: Just a simple list, if possible ranked in > terms of "quality" in someone's opinion. It would also be nice > mentioning if the item is in print. This is reproduced from a chapter in print. It is not comprehensive, but I hope it helps. from: JH Langdon and ME McGann, eds. 1993. The Natural History of Paradigms. University of Indianapolis Press. (Forthcoming) The Darwinian Revolution: A Selected Bibliography John H. Langdon Darwin left behind an extensive documentation of his thoughts, from his notebooks which span more than two decades before he published the Origin, to his autobiography. With the impact that Darwinism has had on science, society, and nearly every other aspect of Western culture, historians of science have examined and reexamined each clue for the cause and process of this critical paradigm shift. The best source for Darwin's ideas are his own books, for Darwin wrote vividly and clearly. The Voyage of the Beagle (1840-1843) recounts his trip of exploration that stimulated much of his thinking about the origin, evolution, and dispersion of species. The Origin of Species (1859) is his revolutionary presentation of natural selection. In The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex (1871), Darwin extends his theories to human evolution. Biographies of Darwin are recorded by Darwin himself (Barlow, 1958) and most recently by Desmond and Moore (1992). Other examinations of the development of his thought come from Bowler (1990), Eiseley (1979), Gale (1982), Ghiselin (1969), Mayr (1991), Moorhead (1969), and Ospovat (1981). Stone (1980) has written a fictionalized account of his life. The intellectual, social, and political contexts have been documented extensively. The scientific history of the period is recounted by Bowler (1984, 1989), Brachman (1980), Brooks (1984), Eiseley (1958), Grayson (1983), Irvine (1955), Mayr (1972, 1982), and Ruse (1979). Desmond (1982, 1990) has linked the popular discussion of evolution with radical social politics. More general accounts of the history of evolutionary biology, extending into the 20th century include those of Bowler (1983), Edey and Johanson (1989), and Mayr (1980). Barlow, Nora, ed. 1958. The Autobiography of Charles Darwin. New York: Norton. Bowler, Peter J. 1983. The Eclipse of Darwinism: Anti-Darwinian Evolution Theories in the Decades around 1900. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. __________ 1984. Evolution: The History of an Idea. Berkeley: University of California Press. __________ 1989. The Invention of Progress: The Victorians and the Past. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. __________ 1990. Charles Darwin, the Man and his Influence. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Brachman, Arnold C. 1980. A Delicate Arrangement: The Strange Case of Charles Darwin and Alfred Russel Wallace.New York: Times Books. Brooks, John Langdon 1984. Just before the Origin: Alfred Russel Wallace's Theory of Evolution. New York: Columbia University Press. Darwin, Charles 1989. The Voyage of the Beagle. (1840-1843). Janet Brown and Michael Neve, eds. Harmondsworth: Penguin. __________ 1982. The Origin of Species. (1859). J.W. Burrow, ed. Harmondsworth: Penguin. __________ 1981. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. (1871). Princeton: University of Princeton Press. Desmond, Adrian 1982. Archetypes and Ancestors: Paleontology in Victorian London 1850-1875. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. __________ 1990. The Politics of Evolution: Morphology, Medicine, and Reform in Radical London. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Desmond, Adrian, and James Moore 1992. Darwin. New York: Warner. Edey, Maitland A., and Donald C. Johanson 1989. Blueprints: Solving the Mystery of Evolution. Boston: Little, Brown. Eisley, Loren 1958. Darwin's Century: Evolution and the Men who Discovered it. Garden City, NY: Doubleday and Co. __________ 1979. Darwin and the Mysterious Mr. X: New Light on the Evolutionists. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Javonovich. Gale, Barry G. 1982. Evolution without Evidence: Charles Darwin and the Origin of Species. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. Ghiselin, Michael T. 1969. The Triumph of the Darwinian Method. Berkeley: University of California Press. Grayson, Donald K. 1983. The Establishment of Human Antiquity. New York: Academic Press. Greene, John C. 1992. From Aristotle to Darwin: reflections on Ernst Mayr's interpretation in The Growth of Biological Thought. Journal of the History of Biology 25(2):257-284. Irvine, William 1955. Apes, Angels, and Victorians: Darwin, Huxley, and Evolution. New York: McGraw-Hill. Mayr, Ernst 1972. The nature of the Darwinian Revolution. Science 176:981-989. __________ 1980. The Evolutionary Synthesis: Perspectives on the Unification of Biology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. __________ 1982. The Growth of Biological Thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. __________ 1991. One Long Argument: Charles Darwin and the Genesis of Modern Evolutionary Thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Moorehead, Alan 1969. Darwin and the Beagle. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin. Ospovat, Dov 1981. The Development of Darwin's Theory: Natural History, Natural Theology, and Natural Selection. New York: Cambridge University Press. Ruse, Michael 1979. The Darwinian Revolution: Science Red in Tooth and Claw. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Stone, Irving 1980. The Origin: A Biographical Novel of Charles Darwin. New York: Doubleday. JOHN H. LANGDON email LANGDON@GANDLF.UINDY.EDU DEPARTMENT OF BIOLOGY FAX (317) 788-3569 UNIVERSITY OF INDIANAPOLIS PHONE (317) 788-3447 INDIANAPOLIS, IN 46227 _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:89>From mayerg@cs.uwp.edu Mon Nov 22 11:47:50 1993 Date: Mon, 22 Nov 1993 11:47:56 -0600 (CST) From: Gregory Mayer <mayerg@cs.uwp.edu> Subject: Re: Momentum and other physical metaphors in history To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu > One that can be followed now is the > drift towards using the plural "they" or "their" in the predicate of > a sentence with a singular neuter subject: "everyone"; "each" and so > on. The shift is a response to not depending on a masculine singular > pronoun--and avoiding the clumsy his/her etc. I frequently use the "they/their" construction when referring to a neuter singular subject. I don't believe this is a recent development to avoid the clumsy his/her, though. My recollection is that this has been a common form for many years where I grew up (New York). Another "gap", the lack of a distinct second person plural, has led to the usage in New York of "yous" (often spelled "youse") and "you guys" (similar to Southern "you all") when speaking to a group (of either or both sexes). Are these gaps what linguists call "imbalances" (a term used by some in previous postings)? Gregory C. Mayer mayerg@cs.uwp.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:90>From leh1@Lehigh.EDU Mon Nov 22 14:13:07 1993 Date: Mon, 22 Nov 1993 15:11:33 EST From: leh1@Lehigh.EDU (Lynn E. Hanninen) Subject: Re: phenetics vs cladistics vs evol. class. To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Hi! > The cladist,... must still make decisions as to which rank sister taxa should be, and which sister taxa should be named (in a practical system, not all sister taxa can be named). This is what Mayr meant by saying cladistics had no method. I'm sorry, but I just don't understand all this about rank and naming sister taxa (to tell the truth I still don't understand what a taxon IS). lynn (STILL trying) ************************** Lehigh office: rm. 221, CU #17 office phone #: (215) 758-3662 home phone #: (215) 758-1367 e-mail: leh1@lehigh.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:91>From D.Oldroyd@unsw.edu.au Mon Nov 22 21:42:34 1993 Date: Tue, 23 Nov 1993 14:53:48 +1000 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: D.Oldroyd@unsw.edu.au Subject: George Maw George Maw was a 19th-C industrialist who had a ceramic tile-making factory in the Severn Valley. His factory is now part of the museum complex in the Ironbridge Gorge, and it is interesting to visit the place: the cradle of the Industrial Revolution. Maw was an amateur naturalist/geologist and did some geological work in North Wales. I have looked at this in relation to a detailed study of the history of ideas about the Precambrian in North Wales, which is being published in 'Annals of Science' this month. I hope this information may be of some interest/use. David Oldroyd (University of New South Wales) David Oldroyd, School of Science and Technology Studies, University of New South Wales _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:92>From ronald@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu Tue Nov 23 00:16:09 1993 Date: Mon, 22 Nov 93 20:18:20 HST From: Ron Amundson <ronald@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Request for Borges's "Chinese" classification Sorry to bother the list about a minor matter, but I mistakenly thought I'd saved the Borges (later Foucault) facetious classification scheme which has been listed and discussed on Darwin-L. If anyone has it handy, I'd very much appreciate having a copy posted to me. I have to give a lecture on systematics soon, and Borges sets the tone nicely, esp. since one of the faculty involved in the group has proposed that all classification schemes are arbitrary. Thanks for any help. Ron Amundson ronald@uhunix.uhcc.hawaii.edu ronald@uhunix.bitnet _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:93>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Tue Nov 23 15:42:51 1993 Date: Tue, 23 Nov 1993 16:49:59 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: November 23 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro NOVEMBER 23 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1553/1616: PROSPERO ALPINI, botanist and physician, is born at Marostica, Italy; he will die on this day in 1616 at Padua. One of the first European physicians to study plants in a non-medicinal context, Alpini will travel to Egypt and Crete, and will publish the first description of the Egyptian flora, _De Plantis Aegypti_ (1592). In 1603 Alpini will assume the directorship of the botanical garden at the University of Padua; his son, Alpino, will succeed him in this position after Alpini's death. Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L, an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. For information send the message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:94>From mayerg@cs.uwp.edu Tue Nov 23 16:03:15 1993 Date: Tue, 23 Nov 1993 16:00:10 -0600 (CST) From: Gregory Mayer <mayerg@cs.uwp.edu> Subject: Books about Darwin To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu What follows is by no means a bibliography; it is merely a selection of the books readily at hand to me that might get Ricardo Nassif started on the history of evolutionary thought. It lacks some very important works: the _Life and Letters_, the new edition of the correspondence from Cambridge, de Beer's biography, etc., etc. Others may be stimulated to add their favorites. Appleman, P., ed. 1970. Darwin. Norton, New York. A "Norton Critical Edition", containing extracts from Darwin's works plus a collection of comtemporary and modern commentary. Brackman, A.C. 1980. A Delicate Arrangement. Times Books, New York. About Darwin and Wallace's joint announcement of natural selection; the author feels Wallace was wronged. Wallace didn't feel that way, nor do most historians. I believe David Kohn wrote a pretty damning review. Out of print. Bowler, P.J. 1989. Evolution, the History of an Idea. 2nd ed. UC Press, Berkeley. A widely regarded overview beginning well before Darwin, and up to the present day. Barlow, N., ed. 1967. Darwin and Henslow. The Growth of an Idea. John Murray, London. Letters 1831-1860. Darwin, C. 1958. The Autobiography of Charles Darwin. Norton, New York. This is the unexpurgated version. Darwin, C. 1980. Metaphysics, Materialism, and the Evolution of Mind. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago. Notebooks, etc. transcribed and annotated by P.H. Barrett; see also Gruber, below. Desmond, A. and J. Moore. 1991. Darwin. Warner Books, New York. This much praised work has previously received criticism on our list for being speculative and overly concerned with social and political issues. I have found it to be pretty standard, actually. I was much amused by a review (in _Isis_?) which thought the book paid insufficient attention to sexual matters. Eiseley, L. 1958. Darwin's Century: Evolution and the Men Who Discovered It. Doubleday, New York. "popular and rather opinionated" fide Bowler (1989:14). Farrington, B. 1966. What Darwin Really Said. Schocken Books, New York. A brief book by a classicist who didn't think much of Darwin. A typical marginal annotation from my copy: "Rubbish!!" Out of print. Ghiselin, M. 1969. The Triumph of the Darwinian Method. UC Press, Berkeley. Argues for the importance of hypothetico-deductive methodology in Darwin's work, and for the essential unity of its themes. Reissued by University of Chicago I believe. Gillespie, N.C. 1979. Charles Darwin and the Problem of Creation. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago. I haven't read it. Greene, J.C. 1959. The Death of Adam. Iowa State Univ. Press, Ames, Iowa. "remains one of the most scholarly interpretations of the [Darwinian] revolution" fide Bowler (1989). Later reissued by New American Library, New York; may now be out of print. Greene, J.C. 1981. Science, Ideology, and World View. UC Press, Berkeley. A collection of Greene's essays. Not strictly about Darwin, but close enough to fit in this list. Greene seems to think Spencer was more thoroughgoingly Darwinistic than Darwin. Gruber, H.E. 1981. Darwin on Man. 2nd ed. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago. Subtitled "A psychological study of scientific creativity". First edition was published as a single volume with Darwin (1980) in 1974. Howard, J. 1982. Darwin. Hill and Wang, New York. Very brief, mostly about Darwin's work, rather than life. Out of print. Kohn, D., ed. 1985. The Darwinian Heritage. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, N.J. A very large collection of essays at a reasonable price. Mayr, E. 1982. The Growth of Biological Thought. Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Mass. A large book by one of the more important evolutionary biologists of this century, covering much the same ground as Bowler, but including more on systematics (to which Darwin made important contributions) and genetics (to which he didn't). Mayr, E. 1991. One Long Argument: Charles Darwin and the Genesis of Modern Evolutionary Thought. Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Mass. Reworked versions of some of Mayr's previous essays joined into a single work, with some new chapters. Ruse, M. 1979. The Darwinian Revolution: Science Red in Tooth and Claw. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago. A widely regarded overview dealing with 19th century events. Ruse, M. 1982. Darwinism Defended. Addison-Wesley, Reading, Mass. The first two chapters are a nice 60 page overview of Darwin's life and work, but most of the book is about modern scientific and political controversies. Gregory C. Mayer mayerg@cs.uwp.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:95>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Tue Nov 23 22:01:38 1993 Date: Tue, 23 Nov 1993 23:09:26 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Computers in Historical Linguistics (fwd from LINGUIST) To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro The following announcement of a conference on computer applications in historical linguistics comes from the LINGUIST list; I thought it might be of interest to some of the members of Darwin-L. I'm convinced that historical linguists could benefit from examining the wide range of software that is now available in the systematics community for the reconstruction of evolutionary trees. There is no reason why programs such as PAUP and MacClade, widely used by systematists interested in phylogenetic inference, could not be applied to problems in the historical relationships of languages as well. (They do seem to work reasonably well for the reconstruction of manuscript stemmata.) MacClade is perhaps the best of these programs for the novice, and it is now commercially available: Maddison, Wayne P., & David R. Maddison. 1992. MacClade, Version 3. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates. (ISBN 0-87893-490-1) Bob O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) ---------------------------------------------------------------------- LINGUIST List: Vol-4-973. Mon 22 Nov 1993. ISSN: 1068-4875. Subject: 4.973 Conf: Round Table on Computer Applications in Historical Ling Date: Sun, 21 Nov 1993 17:27:01 -0800 From: jblowe@garnet.berkeley.edu (John Brandon Lowe) Subject: Round Table Program ********************************************************************* * Round Table on Computer Applications in Historical Linguistics * * * * Third and final announcement and tentative program * * * * December 8, 1993 10h00 to 18h00 * * Professor's Lounge, Universite Libre, Brussels, Belguim * ********************************************************************* The first in a series of round tables to discuss the application of computers in historical linguistics will be held in conjunction with the 1993 Annual Conference of the Linguistic Society of Belgium, devoted this year to "Sound Change", to be held in Brussels from December 9-11, 1993. For information about the conference itself, please contact Dr. D. Demolin (ddemoli@ulb.ac.be). If you are interested in attending the round table or wish to continue to receive (or not receive) email about it and perhaps other mailings about computational historical linguistics, please contact either: John B. Lowe Martine Mazaudon University of California C.N.R.S. Berkeley Paris jblowe@garnet.berkeley.edu ULTO006%FRORS31.bitnet 1-510 643-9910 (voice) 33.1.45.80.96.73 (voice) 1-510 643-9911 (fax) 33.1.45.80.59.83 (fax) For the first such *informal* one-day gathering we invite anyone interested in any aspect of this broad topic to attend. The list of presentations and demonstrations is tentative. While the participants listed below are all "confirmed", there may still be revisions or additions. There is still time for other presentations, though we would be able to confirm any time slots until the round table itself. We look forward to see you in Brussels! PRESENTATIONS AND DEMONSTRATIONS 10h00 to 12h30 and 13h30 to 16h30 (approx.) (Presenters: please plan to bring whatever software you have to demonstrate, and let us know what hardware and presentation equipment you need.) Lee Hartman: IBM-compatible based program for testing sound change models, and a discussion of questions of notation. John Hewson: A computer-generated dictionary of Proto-Algonquian. Jean-Marie Hombert and Joel Brogniart: ALFA, Atlas Linguistique Fang -- a multi-media multi-media Macintosh database for linguistic atlas maps -- applied to Fang, Bantu, Cameroon. John Lowe and Martine Mazaudon: The Reconstruction Engine (RE), a program for checking correspondance rules and assembling cognate sets from individual dictionaries. John Lowe: Two etymological database projects: STEDT (the Sino- Tibetan Etymological Dictionary and Thesaurus) and CBOLD: (the Comparative Bantu Online Dictionary). Boyd Michailovsky: Using LEXWARE on a comparative database -- application to Kiranti, Nepal. Ann Marynissen: Corpus-based research on noun inflexion in 13th century Middle Dutch. Robert Nicolai: MARIAMA, a data base for historical comparative linguistics, currently applied to Nilo-Saharan. Marc Thouvenot: GENOR, a spelling generator -- a means to retrieve lexical items with non standardized spelling from texts. Annelies Wouters: "An Atlas of Old-Netherlandic," an electronic database of Dutch place-names before 1225 A.D. ROUND TABLE DISCUSSION 17h00 to 18h00 (approx.) All interested parties are invited to discuss the day's events, to speculate about future developments, and to otherwise exchange ideas. In particular, the discussion will focus on desiderata for software in historical research. ---------------------------------------------------------------------- _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:96>From p_stevens@nocmsmgw.harvard.edu Wed Nov 24 06:29:20 1993 Date: 24 Nov 1993 07:29:31 U From: "p stevens" <p_stevens@nocmsmgw.harvard.edu> Subject: physics and history To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Taking up some points made this last weekend, I am not sure to what extent Cuvier really bought into the idea that natural history and physics were that close. From Toby Appel's book, it seems that he felt that natural history was preminently a discipline of description, and could not be reduced to mathematics. You could of course suggest the fairly recent dalliance of systematics with Popperian philosophy was an attempt of history (that is, a part of the systematic community) to ground itself in the philosophy that leans towards the physical sciences. Connections with crystallography seem much closer, and not simply in the work of peole like Hauy, A.-P. de Candolle, and Whewell; even Theodor Schwann (in his "Mikroscopische Untersuchungen...", I seem to remember) invokes a comparison with crystals. Maybe this linkage in more common in botanical (or botanically-inclined) authors. And don't forget chemistry, and the analogy between chemical affinity and biological affinity... Peter Stevens. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:97>From ZINJMAN@uog.pacific.edu Sun Nov 28 02:52:02 1993 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: ZINJMAN@uog.pacific.edu Subject: Re: Austronesian affinities Date: Sun, 28 Nov 93 00:55:47 PST Does anyone know of any recent Big Picture works on historical linguistics vis-a-vis the archaeological (and physical anthropological) record that focus on the origin and dispersal of Austronesian speakers ASIDE FROM (1) Bellwood's Scientific American (July 1991) and Blust's (1988) Asian Perspectives articles?? Greetings from a new subscriber! Gary Heathcote Anthropology Lab University of Guam, UOG Station Mangilao, Guam 96923 e-mail: zinjman@uog.pacific.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:98>From fisk@midway.uchicago.edu Sun Nov 28 11:13:20 1993 Date: Sun, 28 Nov 93 11:15:59 CST From: magnus fiskesjo <fisk@midway.uchicago.edu> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: hist of archaeology Dear fellow readers of this list, I have just been informed of the existence of this eminent list of yours, as a possible source of hints or advice on the following problems: It seems that John Locke was among the first Westerners to refer to contemporary 'savages' in a scheme of universal history conceived as stages of increasing Civilisation/Enlightenment. He is supposed to have written that 'in the beginning, all the world was America' (I am not sure where this was written, but I think in 1687 or 1690, any reference to this is most welcomed). Juxtaposing this with the views of human history and cultural diversity of German thinkers such as J G Herder, it seems that the way Herder thought everyone had their own culture as good as any Enlightened European (read: French?) paved the way for archaeology - I am wondering whether Danes such as Thomsen and Worsaae had read a lot of herder when they invented (read:applied to museum collections) the three-age-system and archaeological stratigraphy - if there had not been an urge to discover the roots of their Danish nation inspired by Herderian thinking, and if they had not been inspired by the same sort of thinking to regard even the makers of crude, ugly stone tools (ugh) as glorious ancestors, instead of savages roaming in the dark a la Locke, perhaps they would not have invented archaeology to the extent they did. Any comments on this Locke-Enlightenment / Herder-Thomsen-Worsaae connection are most welcome ... Magnus Fiskesjo U of Chicago _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:99>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sun Nov 28 13:07:23 1993 Date: Sun, 28 Nov 1993 14:15:22 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: November 28 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro NOVEMBER 28 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1876: KARL ERNST VON BAER dies at Dorpat (now Tartu), Estonia. Though he will be best remembered for his work on embryology conducted while a professor at the University of Konigsberg, von Baer ranged widely through natural history and related fields. Moving from Konigsberg to St. Petersburg in 1834, he held various offices in the Russian Academy of Sciences, and contributed to the founding of the Russian Geographical Society and the Russian Entomological Society. In addition to his many publications in comparative anatomy and embryology, von Baer wrote extensively on anthropological, ethnographic, and even archeological subjects, such as the manufacture of bronze and the itinerary of Odysseus. Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L, an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. For information send the message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:100>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sun Nov 28 21:07:18 1993 Date: Sun, 28 Nov 1993 22:15:15 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Re: Austronesian affinities To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro I'm not a linguist and so can't really answer Gary Heathcote's question about Austronesian language history, but I did come across this paper at one point, and noted it down because it made some specific comparisons between language history and biogeography: Terrell, John. 1981. Linguistics and the peopling of the Pacific islands. Journal of the Polynesian Society, 90:225-258. The paper has no abstract or I would type it in, but the subheading reads "Challenging an established idea about the languages of Oceania shows the value of biogeographical thinking for the study of island prehistory." I'd be interested to know from any of our linguists whether this paper was in any way influential, or whether there are any other papers in historical linguistics that make explicit comparisons to historical biogeography. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:101>From Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca Sun Nov 28 21:41:00 1993 Date: Sun, 28 Nov 93 19:44:25 -0800 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca (Michael Kenny) Subject: Re: hist of archaeology I can't speak for Locke, but in the pages just after his infamous "cruel, brutish, and short" comment about the quality of life in the State of Nature, Thomas Hobbes refers to the natives of America as an example of just that condition (not, however, specifying which peoples he was thinking of). His other example of this deplorable condition is that of otherwise civilized peoples who have fallen into a state of civil war. M. Kenny Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:102>From GOLLAV@axe.humboldt.edu Sun Nov 28 23:37:24 1993 Date: Sun, 28 Nov 1993 21:40 PST From: GOLLAV@axe.humboldt.edu Subject: Language history and biogeography To: Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Bob O'Hara writes: > I'd be interested to know from any of our linguists ... whether there are > any other papers in historical linguistics that make explicit comparisons > to historical biogeography. I can cite at least one other recent paper that attempts a correlation of linguistic diversity with biogeographical areas: Richard A. Rogers, Larry D. Martin & T. Dale Nicklas, "Ice-Age Geography and the Distribution of Native North American Languages" Journal of Biogeography 17.2 (March 1990), 131-143. [The authors argue that many modern native North American language families have distributions remarkably similar to those of the biogeographic zones that existed during the last (Wisconsinan) glaciation. Glacial ice appears to have been an important isolating agent, leading to linguistic divergence.] To date, this paper has had little influence on American Indian historical linguistics. My own impression is that Rogers et al. work on too broad a canvas - i.e., all of North America over the last 10,000 years. I suspect that meaningful biogeogpahical correlations are possible with a few language familes whose spread has occurred more recently - e.g., Athabaskan and Eskimo - although the real correlations are between bioregions and the adaptive strategies (only secondarily the languages) of specific migrating peoples. Thus, there seems little doubt that the Athabaskans migrated southward from an Alaskan or Yukon starting point, beginning around 500 AD, through the boreal forest areas of B.C., the Cascades, and the Rockies. However, after settling in to areas at the extremes or edges of this bioregion--Northwest California, the southern Rockies in Colorado/New Mexico, the front range of the Rockies adjacent to the Plains--a number of Athabaskan-speaking groups moved out of the forest and took up quite different lifestyles in markedly different environments. These included, for example, the acorn-gathering Hupa of California, the steppe-herder Navajo, and the mounted bison-hunting Sarsi and Plains Apache. The lesson seems to be that, while some language spreads are correlated with bioregions, languages (and cultures) can also quickly and easily cross deep biogeographical boundaries. So the older and more diversified a language family is, the less likely it will meaningfully correlate with a bioregion. --Victor Golla Humboldt State University gollav @ axe.humboldt.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:103>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Mon Nov 29 00:04:08 1993 Date: Mon, 29 Nov 1993 01:12:06 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: November 29 -- Today in the Historical Sciences To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro NOVEMBER 29 -- TODAY IN THE HISTORICAL SCIENCES 1627: JOHN RAY is born at Black Notley, Essex, England. He will attend Trinity College, Cambridge, and will become one of the leading naturalists and antiquarians of his generation. Ray's earliest works will be in botany, and his catalog Cambridge plants, _Catalogus Plantarum Circa Cantabrigiam Nascentium_ (1660), will set a standard for local floras. He will be best remembered for his influential volume on natural theology, _The Wisdom of God Manifested in the Works of Creation_ (1691), but Ray will span the entire range of historical inquiry from the creation of the world in _Miscellaneous Discourses Concerning the Dissolution and Changes of the World Wherein the Primitive Chaos and Creation, the General Deluge, Fountains, Formed Stones, Sea-Shells Found in the Earth, Subterraneous Trees, Mountains, Earthquakes, Vulcanoes, the Universal Conflagration and Further State, are Largely Discussed and Examined_ (1692), to the history and geography of the English language in _A Collection of English Words Not Generally Used, With Their Significations and Original, in Two Alphabetical Catalogues, the One of Such as are Proper to the Northern, the Other to the Southern Counties_ (second edition, 1691). Today in the Historical Sciences is a feature of Darwin-L, an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. For information send the message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:104>From GA3704@SIUCVMB.SIU.EDU Mon Nov 29 12:21:42 1993 Date: Mon, 29 Nov 93 12:03:41 CST From: "Margaret E. Winters" <GA3704@SIUCVMB.SIU.EDU> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: linguistic metaphors I've just spent a very pleasant hour catching up on ten days worth of Darwin-L (and putting off working on a paper!). There were a couple of postings on physical metaphors used in linguistics - and particularly historical linguistics - that I would like to add to. The term `polarity' has a couple of uses, both (come to think of it) historical and non-), of which one is in terms of the influence of negative (or affirmative) grammatical terms on the surrounding sentence. For example, the word `ever' is called a Negative Polarity Item since it occurs with `not': He doen't ever let me know what he's doing but not in an affirmative sentence *He ever lets me know... (* for ungrammatical) This is an over-simplification since such items often can occur as well with conditionals: If he ever let me know... certain words with negative semantics I'm sorry he ever got to do it but *I'm glad he ever got to do it etc. It is also used historically to describe a situation in which semantic opposites go through similar sound changes which are regular for one of the items and very irregular for the other - but occur there exactly because of the specific semantic relationship: Latin CALIDUS lost the /i/ in the unstressed middle syllable in what was a regular change (sorry - it means 'hot') while its semantic opposite FRIGIDUS 'cold' also lost the /i/ in what is an unusual context because of the nature of the surrounding consonants. At least in the area of Romance historical linguistics this is called polarization (I think Y. Malkiel coined the usage). In terms of force and momentum, the notion of Drift is indeed very relevant. It is not just a simple question of the spread of a single lexical or grammatical item throughout a speech community, however, but also the convergence, in a way, of a series of grammatical changes which all result in one thing. Sapir, who wrote about it first, at least in 20th century linguistics, uses the loss of case endings in English as a prime example, with one longish section on WHOM becoming rarer and rarer - but within the context of English becoming a language which marks meaning through word order instead of case endings - unlike Old English or, for that matter, modern German, which still has at least some distinctive endings. To respond to a recent question, I wouldn't use `imbalance' for what is going on with they/them either for an inanimate or for the third person singular to avoid gender - if anything it can be seen as creating further imbalance (compared to the first person where singular and plural are marked, although not compared to the second person where they aren't). On the other hand, the use of `youze', `you all', `you guys' `you-uns', etc. are meant to fix what would be called an imbalance in the pronoun system where standard English doesn't mark singular versus plural in the second person - but many speakers feel this as a problem and, usually along regional lines, find a way of differentiating the two. Enough - I'd better get back to my paper! Best, Margaret Winters <ga3704@siucvmb.siu.edu> _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:105>From delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Mon Nov 29 15:57:00 1993 Date: Mon, 29 Nov 1993 13:52:50 -0800 (PST) From: Scott C DeLancey <delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu> Subject: Re: Language history and biogeography To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu On Sun, 28 Nov 1993 GOLLAV@axe.humboldt.edu wrote: > I can cite at least one other recent paper that attempts a correlation of > linguistic diversity with biogeographical areas: > > Richard A. Rogers, Larry D. Martin & T. Dale Nicklas, "Ice-Age Geography > and the Distribution of Native North American Languages" Journal of > Biogeography 17.2 (March 1990), 131-143. [The authors argue that many > modern native North American language families have distributions remarkably > similar to those of the biogeographic zones that existed during the last > (Wisconsinan) glaciation. Glacial ice appears to have been an important > isolating agent, leading to linguistic divergence.] > > To date, this paper has had little influence on American Indian historical > linguistics. My own impression is that Rogers et al. work on too broad a > canvas - i.e., all of North America over the last 10,000 years. I think there's a more specific problem that affects the attractiveness to linguists of this hypothesis. The dates involved just don't jibe with linguists' ideas about dating. Rogers' hypothesis entails, for example, that the Algonquian family began to diverge something like 10,000 years BP. But Algonquian is not nearly as linguistically divergent as linguists would expect in a 10,000-year-old family. In fact, many comparativists are openly dubious that after 10,000 years of divergence there will be enough detectable resemblance remaining among daughter languages to show common descent. This need not be a fatal objection to Rogers' hypothesis--linguists' approaches to dating are notoriously impressionistic and imprecise, and in any case we don't know enough about the sociolinguistics of dispersed hunter-gatherer communities to know for certain that the expected rates of linguistic divergence should always be comparable to those that we see in larger-scale sedentary populations. But the discrepancy between Rogers' suggestions and linguists' normal expectations is pretty gross. Scott DeLancey Department of Linguistics University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403 _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:106>From V187EF4Y@ubvms.cc.buffalo.edu Mon Nov 29 19:13:00 1993 Date: Mon, 29 Nov 1993 20:19:13 -0500 (EST) From: V187EF4Y@ubvms.cc.buffalo.edu Subject: Re: cladistics et al To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University at Buffalo Bob O'Hara writes: >Suppose we have three species, A, B, and C. They may be related in any one >of the following ways. (It is important to understand that "relationship" >in this context means historical, genealogical relationship: relative >recency of common ancestry. It is also important to understand that these >trees are "trees of history"; that is, the root represents an ancestor that >actually existed at sometime in the past.) > /------- C /------- C /------- B > /-----| /-----| /-----| > / \------- B / \------- A / \------- A > ----| ----| ----| > \ \ \ > \------------- A \------------- B \------------- C Actually, there's a fourth solution (if anyone's already pointed this out, forgive me, I'm catching up on 3 weeks' mail): /------A / ------|-------B \ \------C The first three diagrams are usually the only ones used because it vastly simplifies the algorithm, although there's nothing inherently more 'real' about them. -Pat Crowe, SUNY at Buffalo _______________________________________________________________________________ <3:107>From RICHARDS@UCBEH.SAN.UC.EDU Tue Nov 30 10:57:25 1993 Date: Tue, 30 Nov 1993 12:00:08 -0500 (EST) From: "Bob Richardson, University of Cincinnati" <RICHARDS@UCBEH.SAN.UC.EDU> Subject: Re: hist of archaeology To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu I have obviously missed something in my reading, but M. Kenny writes: >I can't speak for Locke, but in the pages just after his infamous "cruel, >brutish, and short" comment about the quality of life in the State of >Nature, Thomas Hobbes refers to the natives of America as an example of >just that condition (not, however, specifying which peoples he was thinking >of). His other example of this deplorable condition is that of otherwise >civilized peoples who have fallen into a state of civil war. Locke's picture of the state of nature is very different from Hobbes', since a state of nature is portrayed as a state of perfect equality and freedom. Equality is part of the problem, though, since without concentration of wealth in property, permanent improvements are lacking. His comment on the "Americans" has much the same tenor as does Hobbes: "There cannot be a clearer demonstration of anything, than several nations of the Americans are of this, who are rich in land and poor in all the comforts of life; whom nature having furnished as liberally as any other people, with the materials of plenty, i.e. a fruitful soil, apt to produce in abundance, what might serve for food, raiment, and delight; yet for want of improving it by labor, have not one hundredth part of the conveniences we enjoy; and a king of a large and fruitful territory there, feeds, lodges, and is clad worse than a day-labourer in England" (Second Treatise on Government, section 41). As a closing observation, Hobbes' allusion to civil war and its evils came in 1637-42, with conflicts between the parliament and the British monarchy; the unimpeachable right of the monarch may have been in no small part due to the persuasion of Cromwell, though Hobbes was equally happy with Charles II. Robert Richardson Richards@UCBEH.san.uc.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ Darwin-L Message Log 3: 61-107 -- November 1993 End