rjohara.net |
Darwin-L Message Log 4: 1–25 — December 1993
Academic Discussion on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences
Darwin-L was an international discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences, active from 1993–1997. Darwin-L was established to promote the reintegration of a range of fields all of which are concerned with reconstructing the past from evidence in the present, and to encourage communication among scholars, scientists, and researchers in these fields. The group had more than 600 members from 35 countries, and produced a consistently high level of discussion over its several years of operation. Darwin-L was not restricted to evolutionary biology nor to the work of Charles Darwin, but instead addressed the entire range of historical sciences from an explicitly comparative perspective, including evolutionary biology, historical linguistics, textual transmission and stemmatics, historical geology, systematics and phylogeny, archeology, paleontology, cosmology, historical geography, historical anthropology, and related “palaetiological” fields.
This log contains public messages posted to the Darwin-L discussion group during December 1993. It has been lightly edited for format: message numbers have been added for ease of reference, message headers have been trimmed, some irregular lines have been reformatted, and error messages and personal messages accidentally posted to the group as a whole have been deleted. No genuine editorial changes have been made to the content of any of the posts. This log is provided for personal reference and research purposes only, and none of the material contained herein should be published or quoted without the permission of the original poster.
The master copy of this log is maintained in the Darwin-L Archives (rjohara.net/darwin) by Dr. Robert J. O’Hara. The Darwin-L Archives also contain additional information about the Darwin-L discussion group, the complete Today in the Historical Sciences calendar for every month of the year, a collection of recommended readings on the historical sciences, and an account of William Whewell’s concept of “palaetiology.”
--------------------------------------------- DARWIN-L MESSAGE LOG 4: 1-25 -- DECEMBER 1993 --------------------------------------------- DARWIN-L A Network Discussion Group on the History and Theory of the Historical Sciences Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu is an international network discussion group on the history and theory of the historical sciences. Darwin-L was established in September 1993 to promote the reintegration of a range of fields all of which are concerned with reconstructing the past from evidence in the present, and to encourage communication among academic professionals in these fields. Darwin-L is not restricted to evolutionary biology nor to the work of Charles Darwin but instead addresses the entire range of historical sciences from an interdisciplinary perspective, including evolutionary biology, historical linguistics, textual transmission and stemmatics, historical geology, systematics and phylogeny, archeology, paleontology, cosmology, historical anthropology, historical geography, and related "palaetiological" fields. This log contains public messages posted to Darwin-L during December 1993. It has been lightly edited for format: message numbers have been added for ease of reference, message headers have been trimmed, some irregular lines have been reformatted, and some administrative messages and personal messages posted to the group as a whole have been deleted. No genuine editorial changes have been made to the content of any of the posts. This log is provided for personal reference and research purposes only, and none of the material contained herein should be published or quoted without the permission of the original poster. The master copy of this log is maintained in the archives of Darwin-L by listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. For instructions on how to retrieve copies of this and other log files, and for additional information about Darwin-L, send the e-mail message INFO DARWIN-L to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. Darwin-L is administered by Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu), Center for Critical Inquiry in the Liberal Arts and Department of Biology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A., and it is supported by the Center for Critical Inquiry, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, and the Department of History and the Academic Computing Center, University of Kansas. _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:1>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Wed Dec 1 00:08:27 1993 Date: Wed, 01 Dec 1993 01:16:27 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: List owner's monthly greeting To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro Greetings to all Darwin-L subscribers, especially those who have recently joined us. At the beginning of each month I send out a short note on the status of our group with a reminder of basic commands. Darwin-L is now three months old, and we have more than 500 members from nearly 30 countries. I am grateful to all of you for your interest and your many contributions. In the early days of our list many people sent short introductory messages describing the nature of their interest in the historical sciences. If any of our newer members would like to introduce themselves in this way they are most welcome to do so; others who wish to remain in the background and just listen in on our discussions are perfectly welcome to do that as well. Several subscribers have asked me to remind everyone to please sign their messages with a name and e-mail address, since many older mailing systems show only "Darwin-L" as the source of each message. In the absence of a signature these members have no way of identifying the particular person behind any of the messages that go out to the group as a whole. I have also been asked to announce that subscribers who will be away from their computers during the upcoming holiday season in many countries may wish to take advantage of the "postpone" command described below to temporarily suspend delivery of Darwin-L mail. If you plan to have an automatic "I am not at home" message sent out from your e-mail address while you are on vacation, please be sure to postpone your Darwin-L mail before setting up that automatic message. The following are the most frequently used listserv commands that Darwin-L members may wish to know. All of these commands should be sent as regular e-mail messages to the listserv address (listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu), not to the address of the group as a whole (Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu). In each case leave the subject line of the message blank and include no extraneous text, as the command will be read and processed by the listserv program rather than a person. To join the group send the message: SUBSCRIBE DARWIN-L <Your Name> For example: SUBSCRIBE DARWIN-L John Smith To cancel your subscription send the message: UNSUBSCRIBE DARWIN-L If you feel burdened by the volume of mail you receive from Darwin-L you may instruct the listserv program to deliver mail to you in digest format (one message per day consisting of the whole day's posts bundled together). To receive your mail in digest format send the message: SET DARWIN-L MAIL DIGEST To change your subscription from digest format back to one-at-a-time delivery send the message: SET DARWIN-L MAIL ACK If you are going to be away from your e-mail account for an extended period of time and wish to temporarily suspend your mail from the group without cancelling your subscription send the message: SET DARWIN-L MAIL POSTPONE When you wish to resume regular mail delivery send either the DIGEST or ACK messages described above. For a comprehensive introduction to Darwin-L with notes on our scope and on network etiquette, and a summary of all available commands, send the message: INFO DARWIN-L To post a public message to the group as a whole simply send it as regular e-mail to the group's address (Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu). I thank you all for your continuing interest in Darwin-L. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:2>From SMITGM@hawkins.clark.edu Wed Dec 1 14:32:52 1993 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: "Gerard Donnelly Smith" <SMITGM@hawkins.clark.edu> Organization: Clark College, Vancouver WA, USA Date: 1 Dec 93 12:30:11 PST8PDT Subject: linguistic drifts or "imbalances" The gaps or imbalance you refer to, I believe equal linguistic drift and usually are associated with venacular expressions moving into dominance, aberrant spellings becoming acceptable, influx of foreign words due either to conquest or trade, and coinage for new technology or concepts. These equal the common causes for linguistic evolution in any language. The agreement errors which some associate with the gender awareness caused by political correctness are, as Greg Mayer, points out much older. I've been teaching grammar for 15 years and ran into this and similar errors in agreement from the beginning. The problem arises in your inablity to differentiate group nouns and pronouns from singular ones, especially concerning "everyone" and "everybody". When "everyone" (every person) is one word it reguires the singular pronoun, whereas when "every one" is two words it requires the plural pronoun or noun, as in "Every one of the students." Most folks don't realize that there are two versions of this pronoun, thus the error arises. Other drifts which I've been trying to reverse include: "alot" used for "much", "more" or "many." The use of "its" and "it's" interchangeagly. The use of "ain't" for "is not", "was not", "will not," etc. The use of "gonna" for "going to." Who knows when these non-idiomatic expressions and agreement errors began, but their origins can be traced to dialect differences, and truncations of words and phrases in speech. When we speak we contract expressions, then these contracted expressions creep into the written language. Purists try to stem the tied, albiet quite fruitlessly. It may be fruitful to consider changes in the written language according to these verbal influences. I can't think of the text off hand, but studies have been conducted of these "drifts" by noting their first appearance in written texts. These errors or aberrances are not often harmful, but they can be when specificity and clarity are required. My advice to avoid these comes is echoed by George Orwell in "The Politics of the English Language", by Francis Vesey called DECLINE OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE (1920) and, of course, by Noam Chomsky. Dr. Gerard Donnelly-Smith e-mail: smitgm@hawkins.clark.edu English Department phone: 206-699-0478 Clark College Vancouver, WA 98663 _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:3>From GGALE@VAX1.UMKC.EDU Wed Dec 1 16:34:34 1993 Date: Wed, 01 Dec 1993 16:36:32 -0600 (CST) From: GGALE@VAX1.UMKC.EDU Subject: Science Studies archive and services @ kasey.umkc.edu To: HTECH-L@SIVM.Bitnet, HASTRO-L%WVNVM.bitnet@vm42.cso.uiuc.edu, darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu, sci-tech-studies@ucsd.edu, HPSST-L%QUCDN.bitnet@vm42.cso.uiuc.edu, HOPOS-L@UKCC.BITNET The University of Missouri-Kansas City sponsors an electronic archive called Science Studies. Archived material covers a wide range of disciplines, including history, philosophy, and sociology of science, science education, and other related areas. Several electronic texts of wide interest are available, including Fuller and Raman's STS Curricular Guide. Additional topics include job offerings, meetings, e-lists, and pedagogical information. Gopher service (named after the mascot of the University of Minnesota, which developed the gopher interface) is extremely easy to use. One of the easiest ways to reach the UMKC gopher is simply to type gopher kasey.umkc.edu at your main prompt (=the $ or whatever other symbol appears immediately after you have logged on to your mainframe computer). If your computer service has installed the gopher software, you will immediately have the UMKC root gopher screen show up on your display. Simply arrow-down to Science Studies, do a carriage-return, and you're there. If you find something you'd like to download, typing s will save it to your mainframe account. Further information about accessing gopher service is included below. Recent additions to kasey include the text of Oxford University's Tutorial Guide to the Philosophy of Science, announcements of postdocs in various science studies fields, news of several meetings and jobs, and, in the directory /Other E-sources and Archives, compilations of Net resources for Physics, Biology, Economics and Philosophy. In the directory /E-lists will be found a rich trove of information, particularly involving the activities of HOPOS (the history of philosophy of science working group) and Darwin-L (the working group for the historical sciences). Suggestions or material for posting may be sent to George Gale, ggale@vax1.umkc.edu. Organization officers or other 'responsible parties' (agency officials, for example, or search/program committee members) having material of interest to the wider Science Studies community may submit it to the address below. In addition we *strongly* encourage members of this list to submit syllabi to kasey's Pedagogy Archives. Mail disks to the address listed below, or send a plain text e-mail containing the syllabus to the address listed in the first sentence of this paragraph. Kasey also offers direct gopher link to the International Philosophy Preprint Exchange, at Chiba University, Japan. Philosophy of science holdings in the IPPE archive are growing at an accelerating pace; members of this list are especially encouraged to submit drafts of papers or comments on already-archived papers to the Exchange. If you would prefer to have your work prepared for submission by George Gale, please send a *plain text* via disk or e-mail to him. US mail address: Dept. of Philosophy, University of Missouri, Kansas City MO 64110. E-mail: ggale@Vax1.umkc.edu. For those who don't have local gopher access, it is possible to use telnet to access distant gophers. To use the list below, simply telnet either the alphabetic or the numerical site-designator (col. 1 or col. 2), and sign in using the password indicated (col. 3). The root gopher screen should open display; use arrows and carriage returns to navigate the gopher net. Col. 4 gives the location of the designated sites. Because it is often easier to use a site whose local time is off-peak/off-business hours, you might choose to telnet a site on a different continent than yours. panda.uiowa.edu 128.255.40.201 panda North America gopher.msu.edu 35.8.2.61 gopher North America gopher.ebone.net 192.36.125.2 gopher Europe info.anu.edu.au 150.203.84.20 info Australia gopher.chalmers.se 129.16.221.40 gopher Sweden tolten.puc.cl 146.155.1.16 gopher South America ecnet.ec 157.100.45.2 gopher Ecuador gan.ncc.go.jp 160.190.10.1 gopher Japan _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:4>From fisk@midway.uchicago.edu Wed Dec 1 18:19:58 1993 Date: Wed, 1 Dec 93 18:23:22 CST From: magnus fiskesjo <fisk@midway.uchicago.edu> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: hist of archaeology On Hobbes and Locke, and their view of the original condition of Men, I wish to thank Messieurs Kenny and Richardson for their lucid comments. By the way, I have also located an interesting (or so it seems) book entitled SOCIAL SCIENCE AND THE IGNOBLE SAVAGE by a certain mr. Meek. It discusses precisely these issues and was issued in 1975, I believe, by Cambridge University Press - you may all wish to take a look at it. Any comments on that book? (I have barely started it myself). - However this barely addresses my original question, which was whether one might say that archaeology developed differently at least in part due to the relative strength of enlightenment thinking on unilinear evolution from bad to good, which appears COMMON to both Locke and Hobbes, as well as to certain Frenchmen of the enlightened era, as opposed to the Everyone-has-their-own-culture-and-it-is-equally-'good' approach of Herder. I am still trying to find out just how Herderian Danes such as Thomsen and Worsaae were when they invented what they invented of Danish and European archaeology. Any comments? Magnus Fiskesjo Univ of Chicago fisk@midway.uchicago.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:5>From laudan@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu Wed Dec 1 23:39:03 1993 Date: Wed, 1 Dec 93 19:42:27 HST From: Rachel Laudan <laudan@uhunix.uhcc.Hawaii.Edu> To: darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: NEH Summer Seminar NATIONAL ENDOWMENT FOR THE HUMANITIES SUMMER SEMINAR FOR COLLEGE TEACHERS. June 13-July 22 1994. Rachel Laudan will offer a six-week seminar, IMAGES OF SCIENCE 1789-1914, at the University of Hawaii. The seminar will focus on scientists' promotion of science as a progressive, value-free, objective enterprise, and on their audience's reaction to this image. Stipends of $3200 are available. Applicants must be citizens or permanent residents of the United States. Application deadline is March 1, 1994. Application materials and further information available from the Director, Dr. Rachel Laudan, General Science, University of Hawaii, Honolulu, Hawaii 96822. Fax: 808-956-4745. Email: Laudan@uhunix.uhcc.hawaii.edu. To Darwin members: I have had a long term interest in the historical sciences, from geology (FROM MINERALOGY TO GEOLOGY), to the history of geology, to a current project on scientists' histories of science in the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries. although the seminar will not focus on the historical sciences, it will be deeply informed by my interest in them. Do contact me if you would like further information. Best wishes, Rachel Laudan _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:6>From CRAVENS@macc.wisc.edu Fri Dec 3 14:31:34 1993 Date: Fri, 03 Dec 93 14:32 CDT From: Tom Cravens <CRAVENS@macc.wisc.edu> Subject: Re: linguistic drifts or "imbalances" To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu It may be of some interest to the List that 'drift' is (sort of) a technical term in linguistics, attributed to Edward Sapir, who wrote: "Language moves down time in a current of its own making. It has a drift. If there were no breaking up of a language into dialects, if each language continued as a firm, self-contained unity, it would still be constantly moving away from any assignable norm, developing new features unceasingly and gradually transforming itself into a language so different from its starting point as to be in effect a new language." (Sapir, Edward. 1949 [1921]. Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace. p. 150.) The implication is that 'drift' refers primarily to structural (phonological, morphological, syntactic) realignments--internally motivated for the most part, and evincing linguistic change--, rather than lexical additions from external sources, with only lexical repercussions. An article examining the history and acceptance of the term is: Malkiel, Yakov. 1981. Drift, slope, and slant. Language 57.535-570. Tom Cravens cravens@macc.wisc.edu cravens@wismacc.bitnet _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:7>From sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu Fri Dec 3 17:23:13 1993 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: linguistic drifts or "imbalances" Date: Fri, 03 Dec 93 18:26:41 -0500 From: Sally Thomason <sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu> The term "drift" is so vague that it isn't surprising if nonlinguists find it unclear; it isn't what Gerard Donnelly Smith has in mind, though. I haven't been keeping up with postings lately, so maybe other linguists have discussed drift already. I'm sorry if I'm repeating things. Imbalances (a loose term that is mainly an abbreviation for a lot of different structural conditions) in a linguistic system are, ultimately, things that are hard to learn because they're irregular, or hard to hear, or whatever. A hole in a pattern can also be an imbalance: an example that has , I think, been mentioned here before is the new second person plural pronoun that appears in various dialects -- not always the same form, but always the same function, to disambiguate 2nd person reference: yunz (here in Pittsburgh), y'all (for some Southern speakers), etc. But many structural imbalances are not gaps in patterns. Irregular forms, especially those that aren't very commonly used, are often vulnerable to regularization. So, for instance, most young Americans have only _dreamed_ as the past tense of _dream_; the new past is still competing with the old irregular past tense _dreamt_ for many English speakers. Another recent example of regularization is the plural of _cow_: now it's _cows_, but it used to be something like "ki" (I forget just what the vowel would have been in Modern English) -- or, rather, it would have been "ki" (or so) if _kine_ hadn't been formed instead, with the same suffix as in _oxen_. Still another type of "imbalance" is often considered a trigger for sound change -- things that are hard to hear, such as final syllables in long words which are stressed on the initial syllable. (Well, maybe I'm using the term "imbalance" more broadly than some other linguists might. My point is that the notion covers a heterogeneous bunch of phenomena. There are others besides the ones I've mentioned.) The idea behind drift, in historical linguistics, is this: if a language A is in the process of splitting, or has recently split, into two daughter languages B and C, B and C have of course inherited all the structures of A, including the hard-to-learn things -- patterns with gaps, hard-to-hear sounds, irregular forms, etc. Because they have inherited the same structures, and because changes that are motivated in part by difficulty of learning are often quite similar, we are likely to find a sizable number of identical, or very similar, changes in B and C. This fact has methodological implications. Our Comparative Method, by means of which we can reconstruct (parts of) an unattested, i.e. literally prehistoric, parent language of a family of related languages, assumes -- among other things -- that features shared by all the daughter languages can safely be reconstructed for the parent language. Obviously, the results of changes due to drift, that is, changes that occur AFTER B and C (the only daughter languages of A) have diverged independently from A, are not features that were present in A; so such features have the potential of misleading us into reconstructing things for A that A didn't have. Sometimes we can find evidence that will keep us from making this mistake: for instance, the so-called "yers" of late Proto-Slavic were extra-short vowels that underwent very similar changes in the various Slavic languages. But the results of those changes, while similar, differ in detail from language to language; so we know that they post-dated the splitting of Proto-Slavic into the various Slavic languages. Sometimes, though, we don't have such evidence from differences in the details of changes, and so we are bound to reconstruct some things for a proto-langauge (parent language of a family) that it didn't have. No way to check. And, of course, we are also unable to reconstruct some things for a proto-language that it DID have -- things that have been lost in all the daughter languages, maybe due to drift. Our methods have limitations. But isn't that true in all the historical sciences? Sally Thomason sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:8>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sat Dec 4 23:29:15 1993 Date: Sun, 05 Dec 1993 00:35:44 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Ancestral and derived character states in systematics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro This is a belated second installment in answer to some questions Lynn Hanninen asked several days ago about systematics. I tried in the first reply to distinguish between phylogenetic (historical) inference and classification, and here will try to describe the difference between ancestral and derived character states. Although I will describe these concepts as they are used in systematics, I think the general ideas will be very clear to philologists as well. As always, this is a brief summary; many things said here could be qualified and expanded upon. Systematists reconstruct phylogeny on the basis of _character_ data. What is a character? A character is _an observed difference from which we infer an evolutionary event_. The two "sides" of the difference are the character _states_. (Characters may have more than two states, but for purposes of exposition I will consider only two-state characters.) For example, if we were to examine the 300 or so species of woodpeckers, we would notice many differences among them, such as the fact that some of the species are four-toed while others are three-toed. "Number of toes" in this case would be a character, and its states would be "four" and "three". If woodpeckers are a clade (a whole branch of the evolutionary tree) then it is likely that the ancestral woodpecker was either four-toed or three-toed, and that at some point during the history of the woodpecker clade one branch changed from one state to the other (an evolutionary event). The original state for the clade as a whole is the _ancestral state_, and the innovation that now occurs in a portion of the entire clade is the _derived state_. In this particular case we believe that "four-toed" is the ancestral state of the character "toe number" in woodpeckers, so the transformation went like this: four-toed --> three-toed. This does not mean that there are no more four- toed woodpeckers; there are. What it means is that when we look at the species we see today, any particular pair of four-toed species are not necessarily more closely related to one another than either is to the three-toed branch, because the ancestral four-toed state may _retained_ anywhere in the tree. Thus _similarity_ is not the criterion of historical relationship; it is similarity in _derived states_ that is evidence of relationship (the similarity among the three-toed species, in this example). A couple more examples: In the case of mammals, laying a shelled egg is the ancestral state, and live birth is the derived state, of a character we might call "mode of reproduction". The monotremes (Spiny Anteater, Duck-billed Platypus) retain the ancestral state, while most other mammals exhibit the derived state (live birth). Notice from this example that the designation of a character state as ancestral or derived is always dependent upon the level in the tree we are talking about (mammals, in this case), and is not some absolute attribute of the state itself. Laying a shelled egg is a _derived_ state for the tetrapods (the four-legged vertebrates), because for the tetrapods as a whole the ancestral state is usually considered to be the unshelled "amphibian" egg (like a frog or salamander egg); but the shelled egg is an _ancestral_ state of the mammalian clade. A number of synonyms for these terms are in common use: "primitive" or "plesiomorphic" are sometimes used in place of "ancestral"; "apomorphic" is sometimes used in place of "derived". Thus "a plesiomorphy" is an ancestral character state, and "an apomorphy" is a derived character state. We could speak of the three-toed condition as being an apomorphy of one sub-clade of woodpeckers, or of the woodpecker clade as a whole being plesiomorphically (ancestrally) four-toed. I hope that is of some help, but I know that these ideas can be very difficult to absorb from prose. A few minutes with a piece of paper and some tree diagrams can bring it all into focus, however. One of the most important things anyone can do when studying systematics is to get in the habit of "tree thinking", because all the terms and concepts of modern systematics are defined in the context of evolutionary trees. When you are confronted with a systematic concept or term you always want to ask yourself "How does this relate to the tree?" When told about any organismal attribute you always want to ask yourself "How is it distributed on the tree?" (It might require a major research project to answer that question, of course, but that is the question that is of systematic interest.) The question of how, practically, one distinguishes the ancestral from the derived states of a character is another question entirely, and one that I won't address here. I will however follow this message with a collection of some stray quotations and references that touch on the notion of ancestral and derived character states in systematics and philology, in the hope that they may be of interest to some people. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:9>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Sat Dec 4 23:35:49 1993 Date: Sun, 05 Dec 1993 00:42:16 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Ancestral and derived states: interdisciplinary historical notes To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro Here are some notes on the history of the distinction between ancestral and derived character states in systematics and philology. I had intended to organize this material a bit better, but haven't had the time yet. This topic might in fact be a very nice one for a short historical paper. With that possibility in mind I'd be grateful for any additional references anyone may know that discuss the history of these ideas, particularly in philology. Systematics In contemporary systematics, the distinction between ancestral and derived character states is often traced to Hennig (1966), and while it is very true that this distinction didn't really catch fire until the 1960s, there are several few earlier examples of its use. Robin Craw has written a very nice paper (1992) that traces some of these earlier uses, as well as Hennig's modern influence. (It was Hennig who coined the terms "apomorphy" and "plesiomorphy".) The most thorough exposition of the ancestral/derived distinction I have seen in the early systematic literature comes from Peter Chalmers Mitchell; Craw discusses him, and I have also commented on his work (O'Hara, 1988). Here's a sample from one of Mitchell's later works: "Characters have to be judged as well as counted, if it be intended to use them for estimating the relative degree of affinity between animal types. No anatomist doubts but that Man retains many primitive characters; Anthropoid Apes, Old-World Monkeys, American Monkeys, Tarsius, and Lemurs also retain many primitive | characters. It is reasonable to assume that the common ancestors of all these animals possessed all the primitive characters retained by any of them. And so it is not surprising to find any primitive character in any descendant of a common stock, but there is no reason to suppose that, because any two have retained the same primitive character, they should for that reason be judged more nearly related than either may be with some other descendant of the common stock. Primitive characters may be useful for the description or definition of a group -- they have no value for assigning degrees of affinity. These considerations ought to be commonplaces in zoological argument, but they are often forgotten, and I think they have been entirely forgotten by Professor Wood-Jones in the imposing list of common characters that he has drawn up for Man and Tarsius. Fortunately they have been remembered by Mr. Pocock, and Professors Hill and Elliot Smith, and the considerations they adduce have disposed of Professor Wood-Jones's argument that Tarsius has special relation to the ancestry of Man. It may not be a Lemur, but it is no nearer to Man than to other Primates." (Mitchell, 1919:496-497) Philology In stemmatics (the reconstruction of manuscript genealogy) a derived character state is simply an "error", or more precisely an "indicative error", and the ancestral state is the sought-for "original reading". In historical linguistics a derived state is often called an "innovation", and an ancestral state a "retention". Here are a couple of extracts that comment on the ancestral/derived distinction in philology: "Some of these interdisciplinary influences [among the historical sciences], which are part of general intellectual history, were acknowledged explicitly, as when August Schleicher proclaimed himself a Darwinist of sorts (Hoenigswald 1963; Koerner 1978). Other influences -- probably far more genuine -- existed within the personalities of the practicioners in the form of well-assimilated modes of thinking, too deeply ingrained even to be specifically discussed, as when the same Schleicher transferred the principle of the exclusively shared copying error from manuscript work to linguistics, where it surfaced as the principle of shared innovation." (Hoenigswald, 1990:442) "In 1884 came K. Brugmann's work in which we have the most influential (cf. Dyen 1953, 1978), though not the first, statement of the requirement that account be taken not of shared properties (which could, after all, be retained properties) but of shared innovations -- a requirement, as we have seen...with its own historical interest, doctrinal as well as practical. It has sometimes been held (Watkins 1966; Markey 1976) that signigicant retentions should be accorded the same standing. We must, however, remember that retentions and innovations are not independent phenomena but converses. An innovation is a non-retention, and while shared retentions are compatible with a subgrouping, innovations are indicative of one." (Hoenigswald, 1990:443) References Cited Craw, Robin. 1992. Margins of cladistics: identity, difference and place in the emergence of phylogenetic systemaitcs, 1864-1975. Pp. 65-107 in: Trees of Life: Essays in Philosophy of Biology (Paul Griffiths, ed.). Australasian Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 11. Dyen, Isidore. 1953. Review of Malgache et Manjaan by Otto Ch. Dahl. Language, 29:577-590. Dyen, Isidore. 1978. Subgrouping and reconstruction. Pp. 33-52 in: Linguistic and Literary Studies in Honor of Archibald A. Hill (M. A. Jazayeri, E. Polome, & W. Winter, eds.). Vol. II. The Hague: Mouton. Hennig, Willi. 1966. Phylogenetic Systematics. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. (Original edition in German, 1950.) Hoenigswald, Henry M. 1963. On the history of the comparative method. Anthropological Linguistics, 5:1-11. Hoenigswald, Henry M. 1990. Language families and subgroupings, tree model and wave theory, and reconstruction of protolanguages. Pp. 441-454 in: Research Guide on Language Change (Edgar C. Polome, ed.). Trends in Linguistics, Studies and Monographs, 48. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Koerner, E. F. Konrad. 1978. Toward a historiography of linguistics: 19th and 20th century paradigms. In: toward a Historiography of Linguistics: Selected Essays. Amsterdam Studies in the theory and History of Linguistic Science, III. Studies in the History of Linguistics, vol. 19. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Markey, Thomas L. Germanic Dialect Grouping and the Position of Ingvaeonic. Innsbrucker Beitrage zur Sprachwissenschaft 15. Innsbruck: Institut fur Sprachwissenschaft der Universitat Innsbruck. Mitchell, Peter Chalmers. 1919. [Discussion on the zoological position and affinities of Tarsius.] Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 1919:496-497. O'Hara, Robert J. 1988. Diagrammatic classifications of birds, 1819-1901: views of the natural system in 19th-century British ornithology. Pp. 2746- 2759 in: Acta XIX Congressus Internationalis Ornithologici (Henri Ouellet, ed.). Ottawa: National Museum of Natural Sciences. Watkins, Calvert. 1966. Italo-Celtic revisited. Pp. 29-50 in: Ancient Indo-European Dialects (Henrik Brinbaum & Jaan Puhvel, eds.). Berkeley: University of California Press. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:10>From sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu Sun Dec 5 07:52:35 1993 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Re: Ancestral and derived character states in systematics Date: Sun, 05 Dec 93 08:56:04 -0500 From: Sally Thomason <sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu> Bob O'Hara's clear and interesting explanation of ancestral and derived character states in systematics is easier for linguists to understand than he might have expected, because the criterion matches our criterion for subgrouping in a language family tree: shared innovations provide evidence for subgrouping, shared inherited features don't. Same principle exactly. His egg examples are more fun than our stop > fricative changes (for instance), though. Sally Thomason sally@pogo.isp.pitt.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:11>From CRAVENS@macc.wisc.edu Sun Dec 5 21:03:08 1993 Date: Sun, 05 Dec 93 21:05 CDT From: Tom Cravens <CRAVENS@macc.wisc.edu> Subject: Re: Ancestral and derived character states in systematics To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu One major problem with the tree in historical linguistics is that it can describe closer links than general principles would allow. A grossly clumsy example... Rumanian and Sardinian, both descended from (spoken) Latin, share some striking and hard-to-explain phonological developments. These are not found in other Romance speech types. In spite of the fact that out-of-the-way Rumania and Sardinia are prime suspects as relic areas, there is no reason to believe that the developments are fossilizations of a common earlier stage (other than the observation--used to feed a circular argument-- that they are peripheral areas and do share the features in question). If we were to construct a tree of descent based on these features alone, Rumanian and Sardinian would find themselves arranged in a way that would suggest far greater affinity than is the case. Now, few linguists would want to construct a tree on the basis of one feature, but it is quite possible to choose a number of features (on purpose or by accident) which would still motivate a tree diagram showing commonality of innovations, and thus suggest close relation in linear descent, when in fact the convergence of changes appears to be quite accidental, not even traceable to the momentum of drift. Curtis Blaylock once called this "the tyranny of the Stammbaum", and it's a minor plague in (some forms of) historical linguistics. My question is, how do other historical sciences which employ the tree avoid this trap? Tom Cravens cravens@macc.wisc.edu cravens@wiscmacc.bitnet _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:12>From Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca Mon Dec 6 20:28:53 1993 Date: Mon, 6 Dec 93 18:32:12 -0800 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca (Michael Kenny) Subject: Re: List owner's monthly greeting This note is in response to Dr. O'Hara's invitation that new subscribers to the List introduce themselves and their interests. I am in the Dept. of Sociology/Anthropology at Simon Fraser University near Vancouver, B.C., and am currently occupied with a crossover project between anthropology and social history. To wit a study of the so-called "Poughkeepsie Seer" -- Andrew Jackson Davis (1826-1910) -- a clairvoyant progressivist mystic who acquired a considerable following in the mid years of the 19th century through his "Harmonial Philosophy." When first tuning into to this list, I encountered a discussion of the "palaetiological sciences"; Davis used this term when trying to place himself relative to the intellectual currents of his age. In his "Nature's Divine Revelations" (1847), he said the following: "It is the office of palaetiological sciences to set forth general truths in the departments of astronomy, geology, anatomy, physiology, &c., all as in perfect harmony with each other, and as forming a general and undeniable proof of the united chain of existences, and binding the whole together as one grand BOOK...the only authentic and eternal Book of truths, which is inspired by the Original Designer, the First Cause." Davis's scheme is evolutionary and teleological, from the beginning in 1847 positing, among other things, the mutability of species (Davis says that he was in fact accused of cribbing from Chamber's Vestiges of the Natural History of Creation, and denied it vehemently). Infinite Progress was Davis's theme, and he deployed the science of the day, geology, astronomy,biology, electromagnetism, etc. to reinforce his points. The Spiritualist movement claimed scientific status, mediumistic communication supposedly "proving" personal survival of death (Ben Franklin was virtually a patron saint, and often returned from the dead himself). So, my theme is popularized evolutionary thought in the mid to later years of the 19th Century. I would be much interested in communicating with anyone involved in such issues, or with social historical aspects of popularized 19th Century science in general. Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca Dept. of Sociology/Anthropology Simon Fraser University Burnaby, B.C. V5A 1S6 _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:13>From @VTVM1.CC.VT.EDU:RMBURIAN@VTVM1.CC.VT.EDU Mon Dec 6 20:40:02 1993 Date: Mon, 06 Dec 1993 21:40:44 -0500 (EST) From: "Richard M. Burian" <RMBURIAN%VTVM1.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU> Subject: PSA 1994 To: hopos-l@ukcc.uky.edu, Sci-Tech-Studies BB <sci-tech-studies@ucsd.edu>, darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu, mersenne@mailbase.ac.uk, l-math@math.uio.no, philosoph@vm1.yorku.ca, hpsst-l%qucdn.BITNET@KUHUB.CC.UKANS.EDU For some of you, this will be a repeat message, but it is some months since it has been on the net, and the number of papers submitted for the 1994 PSA meeting, joint with the history of science society and 4S is not yet very great. What follows is the call for papers. Please note the deadline of January 3 for submissions. If you have inquiries about this call, plese address them to me personally, not to the bulletin boards at the address given below. Thank you, and I look forward to your submissions! Dick Burian CALL FOR PAPERS THE PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE ASSOCIATION FOURTEENTH BIENNIAL MEETING CLARION HOTEL, NEW ORLEANS, OCTOBER 14-16, 1994 The Fourteenth Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association will be held in New Orleans, Louisiana, October 14-16, 1994. This meeting will be held jointly with the History of Science Society and the Society for Social Studies of Science, thus offering an opportunity for papers with an interdisciplinary focus to reach a broad audience of scholars concerned with the workings and nature of science. The program committee would like to encourage the submission of papers especially suited to this occasion as well as papers falling within the philosophy of science, more narrowly conceived. Contributed papers may be on any topic in the philosophy of science. Maximum length is 5000 words, counting footnotes and references. If the text includes tables or figures, an appropriate number of words should be subtracted from the limit. Two copies, each including a 100 word abstract and a word count should be submitted in double-spaced typescript. Format and citation style should match those of Philosophy of Science. (See a recent issue for details.) Papers will be blind refereed; therefore, the author's name and institutional affiliation should appear on a separate page. Hard copy of submissions must reach the chair of the program committee by 3 January, 1994. Accepted papers will be published prior to the meeting in PSA 1994, Vol. 1. A finished manuscript (one hard copy and one on floppy disk, the latter produced using any standard word processor) must be submitted within three weeks of acceptance. Notification about the status of submissions should be received in late February. Authors of accepted papers are expected to present abbreviated versions of their papers, with a time limit of 20 minutes (plus discussion). The Philosophy of Science Association has no funds to support travel to the meeting. Address inquiries and submissions to: Richard M. Burian, Chair 1994 PSA Program Committee Center for the Study of Science in Society Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Blacksburg, VA 24061-0247 Telephone: (703) 231-6760 E-mail: RMBURIAN@VTVM1.BITNET or VTVM1.CC.VT.EDU In addition to Professor Burian, the Program Committee consists of: David Gooding (University of Bath), Gary Hatfield (University of Pennsylvania), Don Howard (University of Kentucky), Helen Longino (Rice University), Miriam Solomon (Temple University), and James Woodward (California Institute of Technology). Non-members who wish to receive registration information should contact the PSA in the summer of 1994 at the following address: PSA Business Office Department of Philosophy 503 South Kedzie Hall Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824-1032 Telephone: (517) 353-9392 _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:14>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Mon Dec 6 22:45:46 1993 Date: Mon, 06 Dec 1993 23:52:16 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Introductions, and historical spiritualists To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro (Now that's a catchy subject header. :-) Many thanks to Michael Kenny for his very interesting introduction. Other subscribers who haven't introduced themselves are most welcome to do so. Right after the list began we had a large round of introductions, and it was fascinating to see the range of interests represented here. Anyone who would like to glance through some of those early messages can retrieve the September log file by sending the message GET DARWIN-L 9309 to listserv@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu. The file is quite large, and will be sent to you as regular e-mail in several segments. With regard to Michael's interesting palaetiological spiritualist, Andrew Jackson Davis, it occurred to me to ask whether any of these people who claimed to be communing with spirits did so specifically for the purpose of historical inquiry. My stereotype of these things is that the spirits were supposed to predict the future, or some such thing; but were they ever used to provide supposedly factual information about the past? For example, did any historians try to contact, say, the spirit of Julius Caesar in order to ask him what his real reasons were for crossing the Rubicon? Had he been suspicious of Brutus before the assasination? It is instructive to remember that Alfred Russel Wallace was a proponent of spiritualism, though I know very little about his views on the subject. Perhaps Michael or one of our historians could tell us more. Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:15>From DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Mon Dec 6 23:49:07 1993 Date: Tue, 07 Dec 1993 00:55:34 -0400 (EDT) From: DARWIN@iris.uncg.edu Subject: Different meanings of "drift" To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Organization: University of NC at Greensboro Following along on the discussion of drift I began to get the feeling that the term was being used differently by the linguists and the evolutionary biologists, and sure enough, that seems to be the case. Checking in my newly acquired copy of Raimo Anttila's _Historical and Comparative Linguistics_ (John Benjamins, 1989) I find the following definition: "In linguistic change, an observable tendency toward a goal is known as _drift_. As in biology, it takes a form of complex synchronization, for example, loss of inflection with increased use of prepositions and word order in English. It is also understandable why two related languages can go different ways. If they both start out from a particular imbalance, say, a 'hole' of some kind in any level of grammar, one may fill it, the other may eliminate the odd term. Or they can independently resort to the same remedy, and the result will look as if it had been inherited in both." (p. 194) While the phenomena described here are clearly recognizable to an evolutionary biologist, the definition of drift here is almost the _opposite_ of what evolutionary biologists mean by drift. Evolutionary biologists usually contrast drift with natural selection, drift being a process of random change in the absence of selection, and selection being a process of directed change "toward a goal" (a local adaptive peak). One of the standard textbooks on evolutionary biology (Futuyma) defines genetic drift as "Random changes in the frequencies of two or more alleles or genotypes within a population", and although the term drift was not used by Darwin as far as I know, and although he didn't know anything about modern genetics, the basic idea of drift, as something to be contrasted with change through selection, was clear to him I think. Here's an extract from the _Origin of Species_ (1st ed., p. 81): "This preservation of favourable variations and the rejection of injurious variations, I call Natural Selection. Variations neither useful nor injurious would not be affected by natural selection, and would be left a fluctuating element...." Is there a linguistic term for purely random, non-directed change in language, corresponding to our sense of drift? (Linguistic drift is like drifting in a strong current, maybe.) Bob O'Hara, Darwin-L list owner Robert J. O'Hara (darwin@iris.uncg.edu) Center for Critical Inquiry and Department of Biology 100 Foust Building, University of North Carolina at Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina 27412 U.S.A. _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:16>From GA3704@SIUCVMB.SIU.EDU Tue Dec 7 07:49:56 1993 Date: Tue, 7 Dec 93 07:49:39 CST From: "Margaret E. Winters" <GA3704@SIUCVMB.SIU.EDU> To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: drift I like Bob O'Hara's characterization of drift (linguistically speaking) as being carried along by a current. What is interesting from the point of view of the history of linguistics (even recent history) is that Sapir (1921) spoke of the drift of individual languages (the loss of `whom' as part of the loss of inflectional endings in general, for example), while others talk of drift within a family (Robin Lakoff in her dissertation) or Sally Thomason in her posting here about splits. To carry it a step further, Theodora Bynon, in her book in the red Cambridge series (yes, that is how linguists often identify this series of books on relatively basic topics), puts drift in her chapter on non-genetic change and implies that the term can be used to talk about change in geographically proximate languages which don't come from a common source. As a rule, historical linguistics doesn't like to think about random change and, in fact, pushed by work in sociolinguistics about the non-randomness of variation as long as we can find enough factors, would probably deny pure randomness. The closest we would come (help! Tom, Sally....) would be a class of sound changes which are unconditioned; that is, there are no circumstances that can be identified as motivating the change. One example might be Latin /u/ > French /y/ where the /y/ is the sound in words like `rue', street or like the German `u"' - with an umlaut. This change happened in every phonetic environment in French. However even here we can talk about structural conditioning since Latin /o:/ (long /o/) became /u/ and may have pushed the original Latin /u/ forward to /y/. As I said, historical linguistics doesn't really look at a class of random changes - even meaning change is being studied more and more in ways that remove the feeling that meanings just shift, for no reason, all over the place. Long-windedly, Margaret Winters <ga3704@siucvmb.siu.edu> _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:17>From David.Simpson@bmu.uib.no Tue Dec 7 11:15:04 1993 Date: Tue, 7 Dec 93 18:18:09 CET From: "David N. Simpson" <David.Simpson@bmu.uib.no> To: Darwin-L@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Subject: Introduction I am relatively new to the list and apologise for not introducing myself earlier. I am currently working on a doctorate in archaeology at the University of Bergen, Norway. My general interests include establishing stronger links between data/observation, methodology, and culture history theory/epistomology in stone age archaeology. I am presently working on a "case study" addressing lithic technology using a set of small sites on the island of Flat|y on the west coast of Norway. The sites in question span the mesolithic ("hunting period") - neolithic ("agricultural period") chronological boundary. The study will (hopefully) result in: technological descriptions of the stone tool assemblages collected from Flat|y, intra-site distribution studies (use of space on the site), new perspectives on regional settlement pattern (were the people sedentary or mobile, if mobile what factors affected their movements), and go on to look at several cultural historical problems specific to southern Norway. To do this I am using a method called "refitting" and am exploring the potential of a new approach being developed in France referred to as the "cha!ne op!ratoire" (hope some of those characters did not get butchered too badly). To explain what these involve would take a little more time than I have right now, so I can get back to it later. Just wanted to make an introduction and thank the group for some stimulating discussion. Dave ---------------------------------------------------------------- David Simpson Historisk Museum e-mail: david.simpson@bmu.uib.no Haakon Sheteligs pl. 3 phone: (47) 55212933 5007 Bergen fax: (47) 55322878 Norway ----------------------------------------------------------------- _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:18>From mcglynn@cheshire.oxy.edu Wed Dec 8 10:01:19 1993 From: mcglynn@cheshire.oxy.edu (Terrence Peter McGlynn) Subject: yet another introduction To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu (Darwin-l mailing list) Date: Wed, 8 Dec 93 8:03:17 PST Greetings! I just signed on a couple days ago, and here is the little bit that you might want to know about me: I'm inbetween my undergrad and graduate studies in evolutionary biology and ecology. My generalized research interests include biogeography, the application of evolutionary theory to conservation practice, plant-insect interactions, and the evolution of life histories, especially eusociality. My interests, as they relate to this discussion, are in the history of the development of evolutionary theory - who, what, and especially why. For instance, in the tail end of the discussion that I had seen about "drift", as biologists we see it as genetic drift, a more random cause of evolution in smaller populations due to, basically, genetic "sampling error" from generation to generation. Anyway, Darwin never said anything about drift, and couldn't because he did not even know about the nature of heredity from one generation to the next. He knew it existed, but agreed to a "blending theory" rather than the mendelian genetics that had been discovered, but only by one person. However, Darwin clearly emphasized that there may have been other such causes of evolution that he was ignorant of, and speculated along the lines of drift. That was a tangent; don't worry, it's not a common occurrence. I look forward to some good discussion! -Terry -- Terrence P. McGlynn Associate Student of Biology 7925 Ellenbogen Street Occidental College Biology Department (sort-of) Sunland, CA 91040-2261 phone:(818)352-5242 internet: mcglynn@oxy.edu "Take a page from the red book--and keep them in your sights" -Neil Peart _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:19>From diane@ling.edinburgh.ac.uk Wed Dec 8 10:22:34 1993 Date: Wed, 8 Dec 93 15:48:31 GMT From: D Nelson <diane@ling.edinburgh.ac.uk> Subject: Extinction To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu Before I post my query for discussion, a brief introduction: I am currently working on a PhD in the syntax and morphology of Finnish at the University of Edinburgh. My main research interests are in synchronic syntax, but I am becoming increasingly intrigued by diachronic linguistics, especially in the context of general theories of evolution (this I can partly credit to reading this list!) Anyway, this seems like an obvious place to post this query: In linguistics the term "extinct" is used to describe languages of which there are no longer native speakers. Thus Latin is extinct, and so is Motor, a language formerly spoken in Siberia. But extinction of a language can occur in two ways: either the last known native speaker dies, and the language becomes moribund (as in the case of Motor) - in which case extinction is an event rather than a process - or the language evolves into another language or languages, as Latin did. Because the second type of extinction is processual, it is only in deep hindsight that a language can be declared "extinct". What is the definition of "extinct" in both historical linguistics and in evolutionary biology? Is it valid to draw parallels between extinction of a language and extinction of a species? Do geneticists have a separate term equivalent to "moribund" to describe species which reach evolutionary "dead ends"? At what point can a species be declared extinct if it evolves into another species? In terminological confusion, Diane Nelson diane@ling.ed.ac.uk _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:20>From Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca Wed Dec 8 11:12:22 1993 Date: Wed, 8 Dec 93 09:15:09 -0800 To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu From: Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca (Michael Kenny) Subject: Re: Introductions, and historical spiritualists In reply to the question about whether historians ever used the spirits of departed luminaries in their own research, I don't know of such cases among academically inclined historians. However, knowledge of past events was an essential feature of so-called 'test seances,' in which the purported spirits were asked to deliver "veridical" messages, i.e. information about things which only that spirit could have known about in life. As for the rest, spirit pronouncements generally pertain to things metaphysical, and sometimes to correct the record: as when Jesus returns to clarify what his original message really was before mystics got hold of it and changed it into this son-of-God business. Andrew Jackson Davis's spirits (particularly the character named 'Galen,' the ancient physician) were concerned to outline the social geography of the SummerLand, the true nature of disease, and metaphysical truths concerning the relation between scientific discovery and infinite progress. Always Science is a part of it, since the aim of the game was to establish spiritual principles on scientific grounds. Evolution therefore is no stochastic process, but rather the expression of an imminent telos (isn't that how Wallace saw it? I will certainly check). Michael_Kenny@sfu.ca _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:21>From mcglynn@cheshire.oxy.edu Wed Dec 8 11:40:50 1993 From: mcglynn@cheshire.oxy.edu (Terrence Peter McGlynn) Subject: extinction To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu (Darwin-l mailing list) Date: Wed, 8 Dec 93 9:42:27 PST There is a definite analogy between linguistics and evolution regarding the types of extinction. In the formation of new species (this is really going somewhere), there are two general types of events, called anagenesis and cladogenesis. The latter is where a species "branches off" a currently existing species, while former indicates such an evolutionary conversion of one species over time. In such a process of conversion, there is no well-understood rule to delineate when a species changes from one to another, which would make the first one extinct. The definition of a biological species (which always argued by many, but is the best model we have now) rests upon reproductive isolation -- if an individual cannot reproduce with another, for behavioral or physiological reasons, then they are not in the same species. It makes a lot of sense at one moment in time, but when the factor of time is involved, it's very confusing trying to determine what could or could not reproduce with another organism. I'm definitely not a paleontologist, but it looks like fossilized organisms are called different species when there is a significant enough structural change. However, the type of slow change from one species to another is probably much less common from the "branching" evolution, because usually new species arise in very small populations that are isolated from a larger one... this is getting very biological. In short, when did homo erectus become homo sapiens? That's probably an equivalent question to when did Latin become Spanish. -- Terrence P. McGlynn Associate Student of Biology 7925 Ellenbogen Street Occidental College Biology Department (sort-of) Sunland, CA 91040-2261 phone:(818)352-5242 internet: mcglynn@oxy.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:22>From princeh@husc.harvard.edu Wed Dec 8 14:20:51 1993 Date: Wed, 8 Dec 1993 14:15:03 -0500 (EST) From: Patricia Princehouse <princeh@husc.harvard.edu> Subject: Re: extinction & splitting heirs To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu On Wed, 8 Dec 1993, Terrence Peter McGlynn wrote: > There is a definite analogy between linguistics and evolution regarding > the types of extinction. Definite, perhaps, but very superficial. > it looks like fossilized organisms are called different species when there > is a significant enough structural change. Yes, fossil species are based primarily on morphological differences (Steven Spielberg notwithstanding). > slow change from one species to another is probably much less common from > the "branching" evolution, because usually new species arise in very small > populations that are isolated from a larger one... I understand you to be saying here that anagenesis is much less common than cladogenesis -a view which has become very popular in the past 10 years, but is problematic for analogy with language since languages are never "reproductively isolated" (so to speak) from each other, while species are seen as "temporally bounded entities" (to cite the litany). > In short, when did homo erectus become homo sapiens? That's > probably an equivalent question to when did Latin become Spanish. I see at least 3 important differences here: 1) We know that Latin is ancestral to Spanish, but we do NOT know that _Homo erectus_ (especially in the strict sense - the Indonesian fossils) is ancestral to _H. sapiens_ 2) Even if _H.e._ is ancestral, that doesn't necessarily mean that a large erectus population somehow magically turned into a large population of _H.sapiens_. Erectus (especially in the broad sense) was a fairly long lived species and might have given rise (by branching) to _Hs_ at any time, only to die off later and have its range overrun by the younger species (producing a continuous fossil record in that range but not one reflecting evolutionary history). 3) As mentioned above, Spanish could have been formed by crossing Latin with other languages (surely there was influx to some extent), but a daughter species arises from only one parent (with very minor exceptions of very special cases of retro-virus insertion of functional gene sequence). The analogy would kind of work for tracking morphological change between subspecies, but if one finds that microevolution =\= macroevolution, then it doesn't work so great for species change. -Patricia Princehouse, Princeh@Husc.Harvard.Edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:23>From mcglynn@cheshire.oxy.edu Wed Dec 8 15:16:19 1993 From: mcglynn@cheshire.oxy.edu (Terrence Peter McGlynn) Subject: extinction and speciation To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu (Darwin-l mailing list) Date: Wed, 8 Dec 93 13:15:55 PST Points of clarification: It appears that the analogy of biological extinction has a tighter fit than those of speciation analogies. The two types of lingual extinction are equivalent to those in evolution, although biologists rarely refer to anagenesis as an extinction event, even though in effect it is. I invoked the types of speciation to describe how anagenesis=extinction in both the lingual and ecological fields. Regarding the _H.erectus_ matter, there clearly are better examples of anagenesis. Although at this branch in the tree there may have been cladogenesis, the gradual brain size increase over the last few million years is a good argument for gradualists. Basically, the analogy works for extinction, but not as well for speciation. for consideration: Does gene flow (exchange among groups) have the same type of role in evolutionary biology as inter-language exchange has in linguistics? Terry McGlynn mcglynn@oxy.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:24>From mayerg@cs.uwp.edu Wed Dec 8 16:17:53 1993 Date: Wed, 8 Dec 1993 15:48:32 -0600 (CST) From: Gregory Mayer <mayerg@cs.uwp.edu> Subject: Re: Extinction To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu The distinction between the annihilation of a lineage and its evolving into something else has long been recognized in biology. When a species changes sufficiently so that its descendants are called a new species, the ancestral species has undergone pseudoextinction. When a lineage is wholly wiped out, leaving no descendants, it is said to be extinct. There are some more subtle distinctions to be made (e.g. whether the descendants are one or more species, and whether the ancestor may persist alongside its descendants), but the present one will do for many circumstances. If (as some people claim) _Homo erectus_ evolved into _Homo sapiens_, then _Homo erectus_ has undergone pseudoextinction; passenger pigeons are extinct. Latin, has therefore, undergone pseudoextinction, whereas Tasmanian (I believe) is extinct. Often, it is neither important nor practical for a paleontologist to determine whether a lineage has undergone extinction or pseudoextinction. In such cases, the species are referred to simply as being extinct. Gregory C. Mayer mayerg@cs.uwp.edu _______________________________________________________________________________ <4:25>From delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Wed Dec 8 16:42:46 1993 Date: Wed, 8 Dec 1993 14:40:21 -0800 (PST) From: Scott C DeLancey <delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu> Subject: Re: extinction and speciation To: darwin-l@ukanaix.cc.ukans.edu On Wed, 8 Dec 1993, Terrence Peter McGlynn wrote: > It appears that the analogy of biological extinction has a tighter fit > than those of speciation analogies. > > The two types of lingual extinction are equivalent to those in evolution, > although biologists rarely refer to anagenesis as an extinction event, > even though in effect it is. Actually, neither do linguists, in my experience. It sounds really odd to me to refer to Latin as an extinct language; the traditional phrase "dead language" sounds much better. > for consideration: Does gene flow (exchange among groups) have the same > type of role in evolutionary biology as inter-language exchange has > in linguistics? I don't see how it could. It is more analogous to what's called "dialect borrowing", i.e. exchange between dialects of one language or between closely- related languages. The difference is that you can get inter-language effects between *any* two (or more) languages that are in contact, regardless of how similar or dissimilar they are, and that these can be very fundamental effects that radically alter the shape and organization of a language. Scott DeLancey delancey@darkwing.uoregon.edu Department of Linguistics University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403, USA _______________________________________________________________________________ Darwin-L Message Log 4: 1-25 -- December 1993 End